Subido por Edson Mendoza

[email protected]

Anuncio
Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Linguistics and Education
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/linged
Which instructional programme (EFL or CLIL) results in better oral
communicative competence? Updated empirical evidence from a
monolingual context
Juan de Dios Martínez Agudo
University of Extremadura (Spain)
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 22 January 2018
Received in revised form 2 April 2019
Accepted 26 April 2019
Available online 10 May 2019
Keywords:
CLIL
EFL
Oral skills
Extramural exposure to English
a b s t r a c t
This cross-sectional study examines the impact of CLIL programmes on Primary and Secondary Education
learners’ oral abilities in the monolingual community of Extremadura (Spain). The evolution of the bilingual (CLIL) and non-bilingual (EFL) strands from Primary Education to Compulsory Secondary Education
to Baccalaureate is traced through the administration of post- and delayed post-tests. Results indicate
that the experimental group (CLIL) obtains better results in both oral abilities than the control group
(EFL), with such differences being much more noticeable with time and experience, especially speaking.
Contrary to what might be thought, the extramural exposure intensity factor does not operate with the
expected impact on CLIL learners’ developing oral competence in terms of statistical significance.
© 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Literature review
Content and Language Integrated Learning (CLIL) appeared on
the European scene towards the end of the last century as an effective alternative to the Communicative Language Teaching (CLT)
approach (Coyle, Hood, & Marsh, 2010; Dalton-Puffer, 2011). This
new educational approach aims at overcoming the perceived communicative limitations of traditional educational approaches, and
thereby contributing to improving learners’ overall target language competence. Certainly, such language expectations, which
are fuelled by an overall dissatisfaction with the observable language learning outcomes (Dalton-Puffer, Nikula, & Smit, 2010),
are somewhat ambitious as well as unrealistic (Cenoz, Genesee,
& Gorter, 2014). Perhaps the potential of CLIL in terms of language
benefits, as rightly stated by Dallinger, Jonkmann, Hollm, and Fieg
(2016), has been overestimated. Undoubtedly, what today becomes
‘THE’ answer or solution to language teaching in the 21st century
indeed presents serious limitations and shortcomings, and that is
why CLIL programmes “should only be introduced if the conditions
to make it successful are met” (Lasagabaster, 2008, p. 35) and only if
“programmes are carefully designed and developed in each school
context” (Roquet & Pérez-Vidal, 2015, p. 20), something we com-
E-mail address: [email protected]
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.linged.2019.04.008
0898-5898/© 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
pletely subscribe to. Contrary to most expectations and in view of
the shortcomings recently reported by CLIL research, what becomes
clear is that CLIL is not a panacea for language teaching (DaltonPuffer, 2011) as “there is a wide delivery gap between what is
provided in teaching, and what comes out in terms of learning”
(Dentler, 2007, p. 170). Although the results are very promising
provided CLIL programmes occur under certain conditions, Meyer,
Coyle, Halbach, Schuck, and Ting (2015) suggest that CLIL instruction may perhaps not be reaching its full potential. In the same vein,
Bruton (2013) recognises that CLIL does not produce the expected
FL benefits. Hence, there is a clear need for a ‘reality check’, as
suggested by Dalton-Puffer (2009), so as to confirm the purported
linguistic benefits of CLIL classrooms.
What characterises CLIL more than anything is the existing
diversity of CLIL programme formats and practices (Cenoz et al.,
2014; Dalton-Puffer et al., 2010; Hüttner & Smit, 2014) because this
new educational approach is understood in different ways (Cenoz
et al., 2014; Mehisto, Marsh, & Frigols, 2008). Such diversity or
flexibility evidently leads to serious challenges in CLIL research,
thus preventing generalised conclusions about CLIL effectiveness
(Marsh, 2008; Nikula, Dalton-Puffer, & Llinares, 2013). Over the
last two decades the linguistic potential of CLIL has been extensively discussed and reported by the research literature (Admiraal,
Westhoff, & de Bot, 2006; Coral, Lleixà, & Ventura, 2016; Coyle,
2007; Dalton-Puffer, 2008; Dalton-Puffer et al., 2010; Gallardo del
Puerto & Gómez, 2013; Lorenzo, Casal, & Moore, 2009; Meyer
70
J.d.D. Martínez Agudo / Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
et al., 2015; Navés, 2011; Pérez-Cañado & Lancaster, 2017; Rumlich,
2014; Zydatiß, 2012), although perhaps such claims “are all too
often made without substantial empirical evidence” (Cenoz et al.,
2014, p. 256), hence the need to be more cautious about the purported linguistic benefits of CLIL programmes.
Second Language Acquisition (SLA) research, as Roquet and
Pérez-Vidal (2015, p. 1) remind us, has established associations
between differences in linguistic outcomes and differences in learning contexts, investigating in particular “what such contexts offer
in terms of language exposure and opportunities for practising the
target language (DeKeyser, 2007)”. This is the main approach of
the present study. According to Dalton-Puffer (2011), the theoretical justifications for CLIL have been explained in terms of the Input
(Krashen, 1985), Interaction (Long, 1996) and Output (Swain, 1995)
Hypotheses. According to SLA-inspired research, CLIL classrooms
are expected to provide more input and exposure to the target
language as well as plenty of opportunities for communicative practice, thus creating the optimal conditions for language acquisition
to take place by offering a linguistically more challenging environment, resulting in improved L2 competence (Eurydice, 2006;
García-Mayo & Basterrechea, 2017; Lasagabaster & López, 2015;
Roquet & Pérez-Vidal, 2015). Perhaps such increased and continued exposure to the target language input is likely the key variable
for the success of CLIL programmes in terms of language benefits,
as rightly recognised by Cenoz et al. (2014).
Certain language aspects or areas are developed more than others in CLIL programmes when compared to conventional foreign
language classrooms, with vocabulary being the only linguistic
aspect which is explicitly treated in CLIL lessons, as pointed out
by Dalton-Puffer (2008) and Ruiz de Zarobe (2011). Specifically,
Dalton-Puffer (2008) concludes that research on CLIL shows that
the receptive skills in particular (listening and reading) are positively affected by CLIL education. While Dalton-Puffer (2008)
reports that productive language skills (especially speaking) do
not seem to be promoted in the CLIL classrooms, Ruiz de Zarobe
(2010) argues, in contrast, that CLIL has a clear impact preferably
on oral communicative competence since this new educational
approach provides more opportunities for students to practise oral
skills when using the target language, which is conceived as the
medium of communication in the classroom and not as an end in
itself. In the same vein, Marsh (2008) argues that CLIL theoretically
boosts risk-taking and linguistic spontaneity (talk), among other
aspects. Hüttner and Smit (2014, p. 166) also make it clear that
CLIL programmes promote communicative interaction in providing “occasion and communicative need to students”. While it is true
that the CLIL impact on the reading ability seems evident as a consequence of continued exposure to written input, positive CLIL-effects
on listening are in contrast less clear-cut. Contrary to what has traditionally been sustained in bilingual education, Pérez-Cañado and
Lancaster (2017) conclude that it is productive, as opposed to receptive, oral skills which are more positively affected by CLIL in the
medium- and long term. That is why pronunciation, an aspect on
which CLIL has been reported to have little impact (Dalton-Puffer,
2008; Gallardo del Puerto & Gómez, 2017; Rallo & Jacob, 2015; Ruiz
de Zarobe, 2011), indeed calls for further investigation.
In short, what actually emerges from current research is that
CLIL programmes contribute to the improvement of overall target
language competence. However, Gallardo del Puerto and Gómez
(2013, 2017) remind us that the purported language benefits of
CLIL instruction may not be such because certain intervening variables have not been sufficiently controlled for in CLIL research
(Bruton, 2011). Divergent results could in fact be ascribed to the
differential effects of input-related variables such as the amount of
in-class and extramural exposure to the target language. Although
the pro-CLIL arguments only address the alleged potential or benefits of this educational approach, Bruton (2013, p. 590), however,
reminds us that “there are other aspects of the CLIL arguments
that are much more questionable, or at least debatable”, as for
example the effects of intensity or amount of language exposure
as in some classes the FL is used extensively while in others minimally. Since CLIL learners are expected to receive greater in-school
exposure to the target language as a consequence of their participation in CLIL programmes, recent studies have also controlled the
effects of extramural exposure to English in particular (in the form
of TV series and movies, songs, the Internet and social networks,
videogames, books and magazines, private lessons and visits to
English speaking countries. . .) on oral competence development
(Olssen & Sylvén, 2015Olsson and Sylvén, 2015; Sylvén, 2006),
which is the main approach of this investigation.
2. Research questions
Overall, longitudinal CLIL-research reports somewhat contradictory as well as inconclusive results, perhaps due to the variability
of CLIL implementation which negatively affects the comparability
of CLIL studies in Europe as well as the methodological shortcomings which could question the validity of the results obtained
(Bruton, 2011, 2013, 2015; Dallinger et al., 2016; Paran, 2013;
Pérez-Cañado & Lancaster, 2017). While it is true that oral comprehension and production constitute the least researched language
skills in CLIL research (Pérez-Cañado & Lancaster, 2017), the few
existing studies of the oral abilities in CLIL contexts to date have certainly offered arbitrary results (especially concerning the speaking
skill), as Gallardo del Puerto and Gómez (2017) point out. Accordingly, this research paper aims to add further updated empirical
evidence to the already existing one by investigating the potential
effects of CLIL programmes on learners’ oral communicative competence, controlling in particular the differential effects, if any, of
the extramural L2 exposure contextual variable, thus addressing
the following research questions:
RQ1: How do different instructional contexts (CLIL and EFL classrooms) affect oral communicative competence?
RQ2: Which oral skills, if any, does CLIL benefit the most?
RQ3: How do CLIL and EFL students’ oral communication skills
evolve from Primary Education to Baccalaureate?
RQ4: How does continued exposure to the target language (e.g.,
beyond the classroom) interact with CLIL? What is the differential
effect of the amount of extramural exposure to English on CLIL and
EFL learners’ oral communicative competence?
3. Method
This study is framed within a broader research project focusing
on a three-year large-scale evaluation of CLIL programmes carried
out in those Spanish monolingual communities with the least tradition in bilingual education (Andalusia, Extremadura, and the Canary
Islands), after exactly ten years of CLIL implementation. The main
approach of this quantitative study is to ascertain the impact of CLIL
education on learners’ oral competence at the end of Primary (6th
grade) and Compulsory Secondary (4th grade) Education. The evolution of the bilingual (CLIL) and non-bilingual (EFL) learners’ oral
communication skills from Compulsory Secondary Education to
Baccalaureate is also traced through the administration of delayed
post-tests.
Dependent (oral competence results), independent (CLIL
programmes) and intervening (extramural exposure to English)
variables have been taken into consideration in this study so as
to determine whether CLIL is truly responsible for the potential
differences observed or whether the aforementioned intervening
contextual variable can account for a greater proportion of the
J.d.D. Martínez Agudo / Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
variance. Inter-group comparisons were carried out in each educational stage (in Primary and Compulsory Secondary Education),
namely between the experimental (CLIL) and control (EFL) groups.
3.1. Context and participants
The present study was conducted within the monolingual
autonomous community of Extremadura which is situated in the
south-west of Spain on the border with Portugal and which has very
little tradition in bilingual education (from 2004 onwards). At the
present time there are 293 bilingual sections set up in 253 schools
in Extremadura at Primary and Secondary Education stages.
Education in Spain, which is relatively decentralised from
the central government and thus transferred to each of the 17
autonomous communities, is compulsory from ages 6 to 16, which
covers 6 years of Primary Education followed by 4 years of Compulsory Secondary Education. Indeed, English is the most frequently
chosen (first) foreign language both in Primary and Secondary Education, which is taught three-four hours a week since the very
beginning of pre-primary education.
The start of bilingual education in Spain can be traced back
to the academic year 1996–1997 as a result of the agreement
signed between the Spanish Ministry of Education and the British
Council. Since Spain is clearly below the average for the European
Community concerning foreign language ability according to the
results of the Eurobarometer survey (European Commission, 2012),
the Spanish Ministry of Education has committed to promoting
bilingual education over the last two decades. Bilingual education
programmes are in fact being fostered with the overall objective of
improving second language competence.
As mentioned above, bilingual education in Spain is mainly characterised by the existing diversity of CLIL programme formats and
practices (Cenoz et al., 2014), mainly due to the aforementioned
decentralisation of our educational system, which transfers educational powers to each autonomous community, as pointed out
by Fernández (2009). The fact is that CLIL implementation initiatives are certainly developing at different paces depending on the
Spanish autonomous community we refer to. In fact, there do exist
large differences across regions in relation to the number of nonlanguage subjects included in bilingual programmes and the total
amount of exposure to the target language. While some regions
recommend certain non-language subjects, but allow schools to
choose from a selection, other regions, by contrast, leave it up
to each school to decide for itself. For example, the specific subject of mathematics is excluded in the region of Madrid while it is
currently taught in Extremadura. Depending on the available teachers’ competence profile, each school can decide the non-language
subjects taught through the foreign language, although at least
one specific subject must correspond to the area of Natural and
Social Sciences. The amount of time or exposure to the target language also varies, with most Spanish regions setting the minimum
and maximum proportions. For example, in most Spanish regions
20–50% of the timetable should be taught in the target language,
including English language lessons, but it can be more. In particular,
in Extremadura the minimum proportion required is 20% of weekly
timetable, including both foreign language and non-language subjects classes. Additionally, the foreign language must be used for
at least one session a week in each non-language subject considered in the programme. In short, such differences in the number of
non-language subjects taught in the bilingual programme and the
amount of time that should be taught in the target language provide an idea of the existing diversity of the bilingual programmes
in Spain, as advocated by Cenoz et al. (2014). Despite such differences, certain common features can also be identified concerning
bilingual education programmes in both Primary and Secondary
Education throughout Spain: students generally volunteer to join
71
bilingual education programmes; bilingual cohorts co-exist with
mainstream groups who only receive L2 input in their FL classes;
the weekly schedule and contents of non-language subjects are
the same as those followed by the rest of non-CLIL students; the
number of content subjects that can be taught through a foreign
language in bilingual programmes can range from two to four subjects, to mention only a few. It should also be added that, in most
Spanish regions, bilingual schools are allowed to establish selection procedures to incorporate students in the programme, only
if demand exceeds the number of available places, including their
previous experience of bilingual education and language level. Such
selection criteria will be determined by the staff members involved
in the programme.
B2 level as established in the CEFR is the minimum required by
educational authorities to teach in a bilingual section in almost all
Spain, except in Madrid and Navarra where a C1 level is demanded.
CLIL teachers’ language proficiency level is assessed by a University
degree in the foreign language or external certificates issued by
other institutions (Cambridge University Proficiency test, Official
Spanish School of Languages. . .). CLIL teachers are also encouraged and supported by educational administration to improve their
overall target language competence and methodological knowledge abroad. To this end, both CLIL teachers and learners can benefit
from study visits abroad and language immersion programmes.
Among the teachers involved in the bilingual programme, a foreign
language specialist teacher will be in charge of coordinating the
development of such bilingual programme in the school. Schools
involved in these bilingual programmes are also provided with
native assistant teachers who serve as linguistic models and cultural ambassadors in bilingual classrooms.
CLIL approach was launched in Extremadura in the academic
year 2004–2005 with the implementation of the so-called bilingual section projects in three different foreign languages (English,
French or Portuguese). In 2008, the Government of Extremadura,
in line with European initiatives on language policy, promoted the
Plan Linguaex to immerse this traditionally monolingual region in
plurilingualism with particular actions aimed at improving learners’ overall second language competence. Since the 2017–2018
academic year, bilingual education programmes in this Spanish
region exist at all educational levels and are implemented gradually from the first year of every educational stage (from pre-primary
education to Baccaulaurate).
In the context of the study, only four non-language subjects
(Social Sciences, Natural Sciences, Art and Physical Education) are
taught in the target language in Primary Education, while a larger
number of content subjects are studied in Compulsory Secondary
Education (History, Geography, Biology, Physics, Chemistry, Mathematics, Physical education, Music and Technology). However, such
offer of CLIL subjects may be extended in the rest of the schools
of the region, mainly depending on the availability of qualified
teachers. Given the diversity of CLIL practices suggested by Cenoz
et al. (2014) and Hüttner and Smit (2014), Bruton (2013), for example, refers to the fact that in some classes the foreign language is
used extensively while in others minimally, depending mainly on
CLIL teachers’ language competence and the complexity of subject
contents taught at schools, among other aspects. Although many
teachers have enough command of the foreign language, however,
more specific training in CLIL pedagogy is really needed.
The population sample under control in this study is made up of
318 students from 10 randomly selected schools (8 public bilingual schools and 2 charter non-bilingual ones) located in both
urban and rural areas of the monolingual autonomous community
of Extremadura. Of these, half of participants were 6th grade primary education students, whose ages ranged between 11 and 12
years with a mean age of 11.4 years, while the other half were 4th
grade compulsory secondary education students, aged 15–16 years
72
J.d.D. Martínez Agudo / Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
(mean age 15.6 years). The control group (EFL) consists of 162 learners while the remaining 156 learners form the experimental group
(CLIL). Primary education learners have been receiving CLIL instruction for two years, while secondary education learners have been
involved in bilingual programmes for at least six years. It is a fact
that CLIL learners accumulate much more time of English language
exposure than their EFL counterparts because of their participation
in bilingual programmes.
The sampling procedure employed in this study was probability
sampling (random selection). From the outset of the investigation, homogeneity has been guaranteed in our sample with student
cohorts with the same verbal intelligence, motivation, and English
level for the sake of comparability, which undoubtedly contributes
to the validity of the study. Those schools which revealed the
greatest homogeneity in terms of the variables considered were
in fact selected as the final sample for the study. Additionally, there
is a perfect balance in terms of student cohorts (CLIL/EFL) and
educational levels (primary and secondary education). It is worth
mentioning that no private school participated in the present study,
so the comparison with this type of school has not been possible
in Extremadura. Nor do we have any data from charter schools at
the Baccalaureate stage. Below, Table 1 provides an outline of the
participating sample in this study.
3.2. Data collection instrument and procedure
The cross-sectional data were gathered through the administration of pre- and post-tests to both student cohorts at the end of
Primary and Compulsory Secondary Education, as well as delayed
post-tests in 1st grade of Baccalaureate.
In order to guarantee the above-mentioned homogeneity and,
accordingly, comparability of the participating sample groups in
terms of verbal intelligence, motivation and English level, the
pre-tests employed for information-gathering were previously
validated and tried-and-tested instruments in the field of psychology or language teaching research to measure the first two
variables and by collecting participants’ English grades to address
the third. Specifically, the verbal intelligence test, which comprises multiple-choice items involving analogies, antonyms and
odd-one-out exercises, was part of Santamaría, Arribas, Pereña,
and Seisdedos (2016) battery of tests aimed at a factorial evaluation of intellectual aptitudes. To measure learners’ motivation (in
terms of willingness, self-commitment, lack of interest and anxiety), Pelechano’s (1994) MA test was used to isolate motivational
factors of achievement and anxiety. Both tests were applied in the
same session in each of the participating schools in February-March
2015. Such tests were analysed by a psychologist hired to this end.
At a preliminary stage of the investigation, an initial questionnaire was also administered to the participants, collecting
personal data and information on their parents’ educational background, which was taken as a proxy for socioeconomic status
–SES-, and their extramural exposure to the English language.
Parents’ educational level is identified as the SES indicator with
the greatest influence (Golberg, Paradis, & Crago, 2008). The SES
variable was exclusively measured by the educational attainment
of parents, thus establishing three levels in this regard: low (no
studies/Primary Education), medium (Secondary Education – Vocational Training) and high (Tertiary Education). This information
was provided by students themselves once the meaning of each
educational level was previously made clear through the provision of illustrating examples to ensure respondents’ understanding.
Specifically, the amount of extramural exposure to English (in the
form of TV series and movies, songs, the Internet and social networks, videogames, books and magazines, private lessons and visits
to English speaking countries) respondents are weekly exposed to
was calculated on the basis of their own perceptions through using
a short survey designed to that end. With this in mind, the Sundqvist
and Sylvén (2014) questionnaire, which is based on a language diary
where the respondents must reflect on their exposure to English
outside of school, offer examples and record the number of hours
devoted to specific activities per week, was particularly employed.
The data reported below were gathered through the administration of post-tests (English language tests) to both student cohorts at
the end of Primary and Compulsory Secondary Education in MayJune 2015, as well as delayed post-tests to the same students who
were previously in 4th grade of Compulsory Secondary Education
and who were now in 1st grade of Baccalaureate (in a programme
with significantly less exposure to English), six months later the
completion of the bilingual programme, in December 2015.
Specifically, the listening and speaking tests for each educational
stage were carefully designed and validated for the purpose of this
study (see Madrid, Bueno, & Ráez, 2019, for a detailed explanation of their internal reliability and validity properties). With this
in mind and based on the Common European Framework of Reference (CEFR), an extensive examination of the national Decrees and
the regional orders pertinent to both specific educational grades as
well as a careful selection of textbooks designed for these particular levels of education were conducted in order to ensure content
validity and material suitability. Additionally, a pilot procedure was
conducted, with the tests being scrutinised by external experts who
critically assessed their level of difficulty, clarity and length, among
other aspects.
Inter-group comparisons were in fact carried out across both
types of schools (public-charter) in both educational stages (primary and compulsory secondary education) and social settings
(urban and rural areas). The experimental and control groups were
given the same language tests (one for 6th grade of Primary Education and another one for 4th grade of Compulsory Secondary
Education). Tests were distributed during class time in every participating school under the researcher’s supervision. Participants’
oral communication skills were assessed via two English language
competence tests: a listening test and an oral interview.
While the listening test was administered to the whole group in
one sitting under the same conditions, the speaking test was, however, applied to a much smaller number of students who, besides
taking the listening task, were organised into pairs and selected
according to their level of language proficiency (grades A, B and C).
Overall, the listening test was mainly designed to assess participants’ oral comprehension in the target language in which they
must deduce meanings and draw inferences from brief dialogues.
Specifically, this test consisted of different dialogues containing
true/false, matching and multiple choice questions. The recording
was heard twice.
The oral interview task consisted of an individual speaking exercise (about students’ personal lives as well as a picture description)
and a spoken interaction activity (topic discussion, particularly
for Compulsory Secondary Education students) so as to assess
communicative functions such as describing, giving and justifying opinions, expressing preferences, making suggestions, agreeing
and disagreeing, and making comparisons. In order to gauge their
oral production, respondents were shown several pictures and
asked to describe them and then answer several questions. The secondary education students were engaged in a discussion in which
two topics were chosen to debate from a selection of four. In the
oral interviews, the respondents were organised into pairs in a quiet
room by the researcher. Respondents were asked to interact with
each other. The 10-minute oral interviews were recorded for subsequent evaluation and scoring. A native English-speaking teacher
and a non-native teacher with wide experience teaching English
evaluated such recordings.
While the scoring of the listening test was completely objective
by following clear-cut criteria, the speaking test was in contrast
Number Mean
Student cohorts
Gender
Socioeconomic status (SES)
Type of school
Setting
Male
Female
Educational stage
Primary (6th grade)
Secondary (4th grade)
Baccal. (1st grade)
Low
Medium
High
Public
Charter
Urban
Rural
67
47
(14.7%)
53
(16.6%)
56
(17.6%)
156
(49.1%)
0
(0.0%)
57
(17.9%)
99
(31.1%)
43
(13.5%)
65
(20.4%)
54
(16.9%)
96
(30.1%)
66
(20.7%)
56
(17.6%)
106
(33.3%)
90
(28.3%)
118
(37.1%)
110
(34.5%)
252
(79.2%)
66
(20.7%)
113
(35.5%)
205
(64.4%)
N
M
CLIL
156
(49.1%)
90
(28.3%)
66
(20.7%)
82
(25.7%)
74
(23.2%)
N
M
EFL
162
(50.9%)
84
(26.4%)
78
(24.5%)
80
(25.1%)
82
(25.7%)
N
M
Total
318
(100.0%)
174
(54.7%)
144
(45.2%)
162
(50.9%)
156
(48.9%)
67
J.d.D. Martínez Agudo / Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
Table 1
The research sample.
73
74
J.d.D. Martínez Agudo / Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
more subjective in its scoring, thus following a holistic approach
by considering essential aspects such as pronunciation, fluency,
vocabulary, grammar accuracy and appropriacy of response (PérezCañado & Lancaster, 2017). To this end, a rubric was particularly
designed and validated for the assessment of speaking performance, comprising the aforementioned criteria.
The mean scores gathered in each test (on a scale from 0 to 10,
with 10 being the top mark in the Spanish educational system) are
displayed in the tables below. Accordingly, higher scores indicate
greater language proficiency. The data were analysed statistically
using the SPSS program (21.0 version). Means and standard deviations were calculated while differences between group means were
compared statistically. To be able to address research questions,
a one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) and
paired samples t tests were employed to ascertain the existence of
statistically significant differences between and within groups. A pvalue less than 0.05 was regarded as statistically significant. Lastly,
Cohen’s d coefficient was employed to calculate effect sizes using
Gpower 3.1, as recommended by the statistician hired to this end.
3.3. Findings and discussion
As explained above, the main goal of the present study was to
analyse the impact of CLIL programmes as opposed to traditional
EFL teaching on the oral comprehension and production skills,
providing updated empirical evidence of the purported beneficial
linguistic effects of CLIL. The tables below provide an overview of
the descriptive statistics.
Both RQ1 (How do different instructional contexts (CLIL and
EFL classrooms) affect oral communicative competence?) and RQ2
(Which oral skills, if any, does CLIL benefit the most?) will be jointly
addressed below. Overall, the resulting data confirm that the experimental group (CLIL learners) significantly outperform the control
group (mainstream EFL learners) in both oral skills at the end
of Primary and Compulsory Secondary Education and in the first
grade of Baccalaureate, although the greatest gains are undoubtedly
observed in the speaking skill, contrary to what has traditionally
been sustained in CLIL research.
As can be seen below in Table 2, both cohorts surprisingly
obtain very similar scores in the listening test at the end of Primary Education, as Cohen’s d is extremely low (−0.122), however,
the experimental group (CLIL) scores higher on the oral production ability than the control group (EFL), such difference in fact
turns out to be statistically significant, with a higher Cohen’s d
(−1.184). When finishing their Compulsory Secondary Education
studies, statistically significant differences (on the highest level of
statistical significance: p < 0.001) emerge in favour of bilingually
educated students in both oral skills, being such difference much
more noticeable in the speaking skill, with a much higher Cohen’s d
(−1.489). In short, the results obtained in the present study confirm
that the positive CLIL effects on oral competence are mainly visible
with time.
When examining the effects of CLIL instruction, Lasagabaster
(2008) confirms statistical significance for the listening skill in
favour of CLIL learners. Based on the findings, Brevik and Moe
(2012), Rumlich (2014) and Dallinger et al. (2016) also report positive CLIL-effects on the listening skill. By contrast, Mattheoudakis,
Alexiou, and Laskaridou (2014), Pérez-Vidal and Roquet (2015) and
Prieto-Arranz, Rallo, Calafat-Ripoll, and Catrain (2015) conclude
that receptive skills are positively affected by CLIL but to a certain
extent since the lack of evidence of substantial gains in listening in
statistical terms. Contrary to most expectations that the listening
skill would be significantly improved by CLIL instruction as a result
of the continued exposure to oral input (Dalton-Puffer, 2008), the
longitudinal study by Pladevall and Vallbona (2016) surprisingly
reveals that the control group (EFL-only exposure) significantly
outperforms the experimental group (EFL + CLIL exposure) in their
listening skills. In relation to this, Pladevall and Vallbona (2016)
conclude that in contexts of minimal and equal exposure to the
target language, CLIL has no remarkable effects. In the same vein,
Pérez-Vidal and Roquet (2015) found no differences between both
cohorts in their listening competence since reading but not listening improves significantly. All in all, gains in listening competence
remain inconclusive in recent CLIL research. What is true is that the
results obtained in the present study do corroborate in some way
the assumption made by Ruiz de Zarobe (2008) who concluded
that the more exposure to the foreign language, the better the oral
outcomes by making reference to the continued exposure or input
provided by CLIL classrooms. However, the results obtained are in
contrast with findings published in previous studies, as mentioned
above.
Contrary to what has traditionally been claimed in bilingual education, the situation is different, however, for the speaking skill as
statistically significant differences across both cohorts are detected
in favour of CLIL learners in each educational stage. This finding mirrors previous results from research where the oral production skill
is more developed than the oral comprehension one, as for example the study by Gassner and Maillat (2006) who report substantial
improvements in CLIL learners’ spoken production. The studies
by Hüttner and Rieder-Bünemann (2010) and Zydatiß (2012) also
reveal a substantial difference in CLIL students’ oral proficiency. In
the same vein, Gallardo del Puerto and Gómez (2013, 2017) also
confirm the benefits of CLIL in terms of oral abilities in the Spanish context. The pilot study by Czura and Kotodynska (2015) also
reveals the beneficial effects of CLIL-based instruction on oral communicative competence (in terms of fluency, pronunciation and
vocabulary) in a primary school setting in Poland. Similarly, Delliou
and Zafiri (2016) also provide positive evidence of the impact of
CLIL in the development of the speaking skill. Lastly, in the same
vein as the longitudinal study by Nieto (2016) in which substantial
differences in favour of CLIL learners were found in oral production
and interaction in Primary Education, but not in oral comprehension, the recent study by Pérez-Cañado and Lancaster (2017) also
concludes that productive, as opposed to receptive, oral skills are
more positively affected by CLIL programmes in the medium and
long term. In short, such finding is in line with what has been
reported by Dalton-Puffer (2011, p. 189) “the area where a difference between CLIL students and mainstream learners is most
noticeable is their spontaneous oral production”. By contrast, this
finding diverges from that obtained by Rallo and Jacob (2015) who
concluded that the uniformity of results in both cohorts in terms of
pronunciation achievement seriously questions the effectiveness of
CLIL programmes to enhance learners’ oral skills, also differs from
the study by Rumlich (2014) who reported an insignificant effect
on productive English skills.
Regarding RQ3 (How do CLIL and EFL students’ oral communication skills evolve from Primary Education to Baccalaureate?), the
results gathered confirm the relatively long-lasting effects of CLIL
education after the completion of the programme, as evidenced
by significant differences in favour of CLIL learners in both skills
in the first grade of Baccalaureate, with a much higher Cohen’s d
(−2.045), especially in speaking, which shows that positive CLIL
effects remain relatively observable over time. Or, to put it another
way, the differential effects exerted by CLIL instruction on learners’
oral communicative competence endure even after the completion
of the programme, in the first grade of Baccalaureate. Accordingly,
this result is in line with the findings by Mattheoudakis et al. (2014)
who concluded that the effect of CLIL instruction on learners’ productive skills takes more time to be evidenced. This result also
corroborates the findings from Pérez-Cañado and Lancaster (2017)
who report that productive, as opposed to receptive, oral skills are
more positively affected by CLIL in the medium and long term.
J.d.D. Martínez Agudo / Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
75
Table 2
Achievement mean scores and intergroup comparisons in the listening and speaking tests at each educational stage.
Educational stage
Primary
Education
Compulsory
Secondary
Education
Oral communication skills
Listening
Speaking
Listening
Speaking
Listening
Baccalaureate
Speaking
*
Group
N
Mean
Standard deviation
EFL
CLIL
EFL
CLIL
80
82
11
20
6.132
6.295
4.273
7.075
1.625
0.981
1.992
2.540
EFL
CLIL
EFL
CLIL
82
74
17
9
5.331
7.065
6.588
9.556
2.057
2.363
2.418
0.464
EFL
CLIL
EFL
CLIL
29
38
4
8
4.236
6.616
7.625
9.625
3.250
3.865
1.652
0.443
Cohen’s d
p value
−0.122
0.443
−1.184
0.004*
−0.786
<0.001*
−1.489
<0.001*
−0.659
0.010*
−2.045
0.007*
The level of significance was set at p < 0.05.
Fig. 1. Mean scores on the listening test.
Fig. 2. Mean scores on the speaking test.
In the same vein, Pladevall and Vallbona (2016) suggest that language gains in CLIL begin to be noticed with time and experience,
that is, positive CLIL-effects on both receptive skills might only be
observable in the long run with more intensive exposure. However,
this finding is not congruent with that obtained by Admiraal et al.
(2006) who suggest that the gap between CLIL and non-CLIL groups
does not widen over time, which means that potential differences
between both cohorts may become invisible with time. In short,
in the present study statistically significant differences transpire
between both cohorts over time, as the mean scores obtained by
the CLIL students are significantly higher to those of the non-CLIL
students in both oral abilities, especially in the speaking skill, as
shown in Table 2.
A graphic overview of the testing results is provided in
Figs. 1 and 2. The vertical axis represents the ability scores: 10
refers to maximum test score, while 0 refers to minimum score.
Positive values refer to increasing competence and negative values to decreasing competence. The horizontal axis refers to the
development of learners’ underlying oral competence in each educational stage. The straight line refers to the experimental group
(CLIL learners), while the dashed line to the control group (EFL
learners).
Figs. 1 and 2 show the curves for both listening and speaking skills. What is most striking is that the control group (EFL)
tends to have lower listening scores as they progress through their
schooling years. Perhaps the lower amount of both in-school and
out-of-school exposure to English when compared to their CLIL
counterparts could be one the reasons behind this result. However,
it is surprising that in Baccalaureate the EFL cohort behaves more
positively than the CLIL group in terms of speaking. In short, the two
curves showing the speaking test are more or less parallel, which
means that both cohorts behave in a similar way, although it is a
fact that the experimental group (CLIL) is superior in both oral skills
in each educational stage.
All in all, statistical analysis allows us to conclude that bilingual learners’ English oral comprehension and production abilities
are positively affected by CLIL programmes. Specifically, the results
reveal that the effects of the independent variable (CLIL instruction)
are substantial on the dependent variable (English oral competence
results, particularly speaking), especially at the end of Compulsory Secondary Education and in the first grade of Baccalaureate.
Such finding thus suggests that positive CLIL effects remain visible
with time and experience. This result tallies with previous studies reported in the literature review, endorsing for example the
findings by Pladevall and Vallbona (2016) and Pérez-Cañado and
Lancaster (2017).
Turning now to the last research question of our study (RQ4:
How does continued exposure to the target language (e.g., beyond
the classroom) interact with CLIL? What is the differential effect
of the amount of extramural exposure to English on CLIL and
EFL learners’ oral communicative competence?), as shown in
Table 3, although no statistically significant differences were found
between both cohorts in Primary Education, it can be seen, taking
a closer look at the results, that Secondary Education CLIL students appear to be more exposed to English outside of school when
compared to their EFL counterparts, which is congruent with the
findings gathered by Sylvén (2006), Olssen and Sylvén (2015).
As can be seen in Tables 4 and 5, it can be concluded that the
effects of the amount of out-of-school exposure to English on both
oral abilities in particular are hardly noticeable in both cohorts at
the end of Primary Education and, accordingly, results are inconclusive in terms of statistical significance. When finishing their
Compulsory Secondary Education studies, however, the differential effects of the extramural L2 exposure variable on both oral skills
become somewhat more visible but in both cohorts (in listening for
CLIL learners and, surprisingly, with some statistical significance in
speaking for EFL learners – with a much higher Cohen’s d (−0.993).
This finding is partially congruent with that obtained by Olssen
and Sylvén (2015) who concluded in particular that extramural
English does not seem to have any significant impact on progress
of academic vocabulary over time.
76
J.d.D. Martínez Agudo / Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
Table 3
Number of hours of extramural English per week according to educational level.
Educational level
Group
Mean
Standard deviation
p value
Primary
Education
Compulsory
Secondary Education
EFL
CLIL
EFL
CLIL
10.30
11.03
17.75
22.08
22.67
14.59
29.57
26.14
0.102
*
0.006*
The level of significance was set at p < 0.05.
Table 4
Listening results in terms of extramural exposure to English according to educational level and group.
Listening
Educational level
Group
CLIL
Primary Education
EFL
Compulsory
Secondary
Education
CLIL
EFL
Extramural exposure to English (average hours per week)
N
Mean
Standard deviation
≤9 hours
>9 hours
≤9 hours
>9 hours
51
31
60
20
6.18
6.49
5.95
6.69
1.18
0.48
1.70
1.25
≤9 hours
>9 hours
≤9 hours
>9 hours
22
52
40
42
6.30
7.39
5.04
5.61
2.78
2.11
2.07
2.03
Extramural exposure to English (average hours per week)
N
Mean
Standard deviation
≤9 hours
>9 hours
≤9 hours
>9 hours
12
8
9
2
7.13
7.00
4.28
4.25
2.58
2.66
2.14
1.77
≤9 hours
>9 hours
≤9 hours
>9 hours
1
8
9
8
9.00
9.63
5.56
7.75
0.44
2.71
1.44
Cohen’s d
p value
−0.324
0.159
−0.461
0.078
−0.469
0.069
−0.281
0.207
Cohen’s d
p value
0.048
0.918
0.013
0.987
−0.993
0.055*
*The level of significance was set at p < 0.05.
Table 5
Speaking results in terms of extramural exposure to English according to educational level and group.
Speaking
Educational level
Group
CLIL
Primary Education
EFL
Compulsory
Secondary
Education
*
CLIL
EFL
The level of significance was set at p < 0.05.
4. Conclusion
Given that current CLIL research offers contradictory as well as
inconclusive results in showing variability (Bruton, 2015) in their
findings and in view of the scarcity of research into the impact of
CLIL programmes on oral competence, this cross-sectional study
aims at adding further updated empirical evidence to the already
existing one by reporting on the outcomes of the potential effects
of CLIL instruction on learners’ developing oral competence. More
specifically, the differential effects of two different instructional
contexts (EFL and CLIL programmes) on learners’ oral comprehension and production competence have been examined and
contrasted. Based on the findings, it can be concluded that greater
exposure to the foreign language as a result of the participation
in CLIL programmes results in a more developed oral competence, which is in line with Ruiz de Zarobe (2008). In relation to
this, Gallardo del Puerto and Gómez (2017) made it clear that the
exposure intensity factor might be operating. Overall, the results
show that the experimental group (CLIL) obtains better results
than the control group (EFL) on both oral communication skills,
being such differences statistically significant and, most importantly, noticeable with time and experience, mostly when finishing
their Compulsory Secondary Education studies and even at followup six months later after the completion of the programme (in
the first grade of Baccalaureate), especially the speaking skill. Contrary to what has been traditionally assumed, this finding rejects
the hypothesis that CLIL instruction develops more listening than
speaking as a result of continued exposure to the target language
oral input. Perhaps the results obtained in the present study for
the speaking skill may be inconclusive due mainly to the fact that
its scoring is generally more subjective and, therefore, findings
in terms of statistical significance are relatively questionable and,
consequently, should be addressed with caution. What is most
striking about this finding, at least in the context under control
in this study, is that while success in oral skills is mainly ascribed
to the potential of CLIL programmes per se (in terms of continued
exposure to L2 input inside the classroom), the extramural exposure intensity factor does not operate with the expected impact
on CLIL learners’ developing oral competence in terms of statistical
significance. Accordingly, the contradictory results of current CLIL
research may in fact lead us to conclude that the particularity of CLIL
contexts and conditions under which such programmes are actually implemented may account for the differences observed across
both cohorts regarding the linguistic benefits. Or, to put it another
way, the contradictory results obtained from the different studies
to date are probably related to the variability in the implementation
of CLIL programmes (Bruton, 2015).
The findings from this quantitative cross-sectional study should
be interpreted with caution not only for its limitations but also
because the effects of other possible factors affecting the results
obtained have not been considered. Given the limited sample size
(in particular, the Baccalaureate sample), the geographical area that
this study covers as well as the idiosyncrasies of the CLIL approach
in Extremadura, what becomes clear is that the findings might not
J.d.D. Martínez Agudo / Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
be transferable to other educational settings. Despite these limitations, this study provides updated empirical evidence of the
positive effects of CLIL programmes on the learners’ developing oral
competence, whilst filling a void in the existing literature on CLIL by
expanding its discussion to an area which has hardly been explored
so far: the relationship or interaction between CLIL education and
input-related variables such as extramural exposure to English.
Without any doubt, the effectiveness of CLIL instruction depends
largely on countless factors, as for example the intensity of exposure to the target language as well as CLIL teachers’ language
proficiency, to mention just a few. In my opinion, a higher level
of language proficiency should be required to teach in bilingual
programmes. With this in mind, future studies should address the
effects of out-of-school exposure on learners’ language competence
more in depth, in particular the intensity and the conditions of
exposure to the target language. Or, to put it another way, the quantity and quality of the oral input learners are exposed to should
be examined in detail in future investigation (Rallo & Jacob, 2015).
Lastly, as Cenoz et al. (2014, p. 256) rightly suggest, “more balanced
reflection on both the strengths and shortcomings or gaps in our
understanding of CLIL and its effectiveness in diverse contexts” is
actually needed so as to better understand the differential impact
of the CLIL approach on the overall target language competence.
Funding
This study derived from two governmentally-funded research
projects financed by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness [research grant number FFI2012-32221] and the
Government of Andalusia (Spain) [research grant number P12HUM-2348] (Project: The Effects of Content and Language Integrated
Learning in Monolingual Communities: A Large-Scale Evaluation).
References
Admiraal, W., Westhoff, G., & de Bot, K. (2006). Evaluation of bilingual secondary education in the Netherlands: Students’ language proficiency in English. Educational
Research and Evaluation, 12(1), 75–93.
Brevik, L. M., & Moe, E. (2012). Effects of CLIL teaching on language outcomes. In D.
Tsagari, & I. Csépes (Eds.), Collaboration in language testing and assessment (pp.
213–227). Bern: Peter Lang.
Bruton, A. (2011). Is CLIL so beneficial, or just selective? Re-evaluating some of the
research. System, 39, 523–532.
Bruton, A. (2013). CLIL: Some of the reasons why. . . and why not. System, 41,
587–597.
Bruton, A. (2015). CLIL: Detail matters in the whole picture. More than a reply to J.
Hüttner and U. Smit (2014). System, 53, 119–128.
Cenoz, J., Genesee, F., & Gorter, D. (2014). Critical analysis of CLIL: Taking stock and
looking forward. Applied Linguistics, 35(3), 243–262.
Coral, J., Lleixà, T., & Ventura, C. (2016). Foreign language competence and content
and language integrated learning in multilingual schools in Catalonia: An ex post
facto study analysing the results of state key competences testing. International
Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 1–12.
Coyle, D. (2007). Content and language integrated learning: Towards a connected
research agenda for CLIL pedagogies. The International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 543–562.
Coyle, D., Hood, P., & Marsh, D. (2010). CLIL: Content and language integrated learning.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Czura, A., & Kotodynska, A. (2015). CLIL instruction and oral communicative competence in a primary school setting. In K. Ożańska-Ponikwia, & B. Loranc-Paszylk
(Eds.), Cross-cultural perspectives on bilingualism and bilingual education (pp.
123–153). Wydawnictwo Naukowe Akademi Techniczno-Humanistycznej.
Dallinger, S., Jonkmann, K., Hollm, J., & Fieg, Ch. (2016). The effects of content and
language integrated learning on students’ English and history competences –
Killing two birds with one stone? Learning and Instruction, 41, 23–31.
Dalton-Puffer, C. (2008). Outcomes and processes in Content and Language Integrated Learning (CLIL): Current research from Europe. In W. Delanoy, & L.
Volkmann (Eds.), Future perspectives for English language teaching (pp. 139–157).
Heidelberg: Universitätsverlag Winter.
Dalton-Puffer, C. (2009). Communicative competence and the CLIL lesson. In Y. Ruiz
de Zarobe, & R. M. Jiménez (Eds.), Content and language integrated learning: Evidence from research in Europe (pp. 197–214). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Dalton-Puffer, C. (2011). Content-and-language integrated learning: From practice
to principles? Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 31, 182–204.
77
Dalton-Puffer, C., Nikula, T., & Smit, U. (2010). Language use and language learning in
CLIL: Current findings and contentious issues. In C. Dalton-Puffer, T. Nikula, & U.
Smit (Eds.), Language use and language learning in CLIL classrooms (pp. 279–292).
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Delliou, A., & Zafiri, M. (2016). Developing the speaking skills of students through
CLIL: A case of sixth grade Primary School students in Greece. EIIC – Electronic
International Interdisciplinary Conference, 5(1).
Dentler, S. (2007). Sweden. In A. Maljers, D. Marsh, & D. Wolff (Eds.), Windows on CLIL:
Content and language integrated learning in the European spotlight (pp. 166–171).
The Hague: The European Platform for Dutch Education.
European Commission. (2012). Europeans and their languages. Special Eurobarometer
386. Brussels: European Commission.
Eurydice. (2006). Content and language integrated learning (CLIL) at school in Europe.
Brussels: Eurydice.
Fernández, A. (2009). Spanish CLIL: Research and official actions. In Y. Ruiz de Zarobe,
& R. M. Jiménez Catalán (Eds.), Content and language integrated learning. Evidence
from research in Europe (pp. 3–21). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Gallardo del Puerto, F., & Gómez, E. (2013). English oral skills in CLIL and non-CLIL
learners: An attempt to control for exposure. In Applied Linguistics Perspectives
on Content and Language Integrated Learning (ALP-CLIL) in Madrid, Spain.
Gallardo del Puerto, F., & Gómez, E. (2017). Oral production outcomes in CLIL: An
attempt to manage amount of exposure. European Journal of Applied Linguistics,
5(1), 31–54.
García-Mayo, M. a . P., & Basterrechea, M. (2017). CLIL and SLA. Insights from an
interactionist perspective. In A. Llinares, & T. Morton (Eds.), Applied linguistics
perspectives on CLIL (pp. 33–50). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Gassner, D., & Maillat, D. (2006). Spoken competence in CLIL: A pragmatic take on
recent Swiss data. In C. Dalton-Puffer, & T. Nikula (Eds.), Current research on CLIL.
VIEWZ, Vienna English working papers, 15(3) (pp. 15–22).
Golberg, H., Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2008). Lexical acquisition over time in minority
first language children learning English as a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 1–25.
Hüttner, J., & Rieder-Bünemann, A. (2010). A cross-sectional analysis of oral narratives by children with CLIL and non-CLIL instruction. In C. Dalton-Puffer, T.
Nikula, & U. Smit (Eds.), Language use and language learning in CLIL classrooms
(pp. 61–80). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Hüttner, J., & Smit, U. (2014). CLIL (Content and Language Integrated Learning): The
bigger picture. A response to A. Bruton. 2013. CLIL: Some of the reasons why . . .
and why not. System, 44(2), 160–167 (System 41 (2013): 587–597).
Krashen, S. (1985). The input hypothesis. Issues and implications. London, UK: Longman.
Lasagabaster, D. (2008). Foreign language competence in content and language integrated courses. The Open Applied Linguistics Journal, 1, 30–41.
Lasagabaster, D., & López, R. (2015). The impact of type of approach (CLIL versus
EFL) and methodology (book-based versus project work) on motivation. Porta
Linguarum, 23, 41–57.
Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W. C. Ritchie, & T. K. Bahtia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition
(pp. 413–468). New York: Academic Press.
Lorenzo, F., Casal, S., & Moore, P. (2009). The effects of Content and Language Integrated Learning in European education: Key findings from the
Andalusian bilingual sections evaluation project. Applied Linguistics, 31(3),
418–442.
Madrid, D., Bueno, A., & Ráez, J. (2019). Investigating the effects of CLIL on language
attainment: Instrument design and validation. In M. L. Pérez Cañado (Ed.), Content and Language lntegrated Learning in monolingual settings: New insights from
the Spanish context. Amsterdam: Springer (in press).
Marsh, D. (2008). Language awareness and CLIL. In J. Cenoz, & N. H. Hornberger
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of language and education, Vol. 6: Knowledge about Language
(pp. 233–246). New York: Springer.
Mattheoudakis, M., Alexiou, T., & Laskaridou, C. (2014). To CLIL or not to CLIL?
The case of the 3rd experimental primary school in Evosmos. In N. Lavidas, T.
Alexiou, & A. M. Sougari (Eds.), Major trends in theoretical and applied linguistics: Selected papers from the 20th International Symposium from Theoretical and
Applied Linguistics (pp. 215–233). London: DeGruyter Versitas Publications.
Mehisto, P., Marsh, D., & Frigols, M. J. (2008). Uncovering CLIL: Content and Language
Integrated Learning in bilingual and multilingual education. Oxford: Macmillan
Education.
Meyer, O., Coyle, D., Halbach, A., Schuck, K., & Ting, T. (2015). A pluriliteracies
approach to content and language integrated learning – Mapping learner progressions in knowledge construction and meaning-making. Language, Culture
and Curriculum, 28(1), 41–57.
Navés, T. (2011). How promising are the results of integrating content and language
for EFL writing and overall EFL proficiency? In Y. Ruiz de Zarobe, J. Sierra, & F.
Gallardo del Puerto (Eds.), Content and foreign language integrated learning (pp.
103–128). Bern: Peter Lang.
Nieto, E. (2016). The impact of CLIL on the acquisition of L2 competences and skills
in primary education. International Journal of English Studies, 16(2), 81–101.
Nikula, T., Dalton-Puffer, C., & Llinares, A. (2013). CLIL classroom discourse. Research
from Europe. Journal of Immersion and Content-Based Language Education, 1(1),
70–100.
Olssen, E., & Sylvén, L. K. (2015). Extramural English and academic vocabulary. A
longitudinal study of CLIL and non-CLIL students in Sweden. Apples – Journal of
Applied Language Studies, 9(2), 77–103.
Paran, A. (2013). Content and language integrated learning: Panacea or policy borrowing myth? Applied Linguistics Review, 4(2), 317–342.
78
J.d.D. Martínez Agudo / Linguistics and Education 51 (2019) 69–78
Pelechano, V. (1994). . pp. 71–72. Prueba MA. Análisis y Modificación de la Conducta
(Vol. 20).
Pérez-Cañado, M. L., & Lancaster, N. K. (2017). The effects of CLIL on oral comprehension and production: A longitudinal case study. Language, Culture and Curriculum,
30(3), 300–316.
Pérez-Vidal, C., & Roquet, H. (2015). CLIL in context: Profiling language abilities. In
M. Juan-Garau, & J. Salazar-Noguera (Eds.), Content-based language learning in
multilingual educational environments (pp. 237–255). Amsterdam: Springer.
Pladevall, E., & Vallbona, A. (2016). CLIL in minimal input contexts: A longitudinal
study of primary school learners’ receptive skills. System, 58, 37–48.
Prieto-Arranz, J. I., Rallo, L., Calafat-Ripoll, C., & Catrain, M. (2015). Testing progress
on receptive skills in CLIL and non-CLIL contexts. In M. Juan-Garau, & J.
Salazar-Noguera (Eds.), Content-based language learning in multilingual educational environments (pp. 123–137). Amsterdam: Springer.
Rallo, L., & Jacob, K. (2015). Does CLIL enhance oral skills? Fluency and pronunciation errors by Spanish-Catalan learners of English. In M. Juan-Garau, & J.
Salazar-Noguera (Eds.), Content-based language learning in multilingual educational environments (pp. 163–177). Amsterdam: Springer.
Roquet, H., & Pérez-Vidal, C. (2015). Do productive skills improve in content and
language integrated learning contexts? The case of writing. Applied Linguistics,
1–24.
Ruiz de Zarobe, Y. (2008). CLIL and foreign language learning: A longitudinal study
in the Basque country. International CLIL Research Journal, 1(1), 60–73.
Ruiz de Zarobe, Y. (2010). Written production and CLIL: An empirical study. In C.
Dalton-Puffer, T. Nikula, & U. Smit (Eds.), Language use and language learning in
CLIL classrooms (pp. 191–210). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Ruiz de Zarobe, Y. (2011). Which language competencies benefit from CLIL? An
insight into Applied Linguistic research. In Y. Ruiz de Zarobe, J. Sierra, & F.
Gallardo del Puerto (Eds.), Content and foreign language integrated learning (pp.
129–153). Bern: Peter Lang.
Rumlich, D. (2014). Gauging the CLIL effect: Results from a large-scale longitudinal
study on CLIL programmes at German secondary schools. In Paper presented at
the AILA world conference of Applied Linguistics in Brisbane.
Santamaría, P., Arribas, D., Pereña, J., & Seisdedos, N. (2016). EFAI. Evaluación Factorial
de las Aptitudes Intelectuales. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
Sundqvist, P., & Sylvén, L. K. (2014). Language-related computer use: Focus on young
L2 English learners in Sweden. ReCALL, 26(1), 3–20.
Swain, M. (1995). Three functions of output in second language learning. In G. Cook,
& B. Seidlhofer (Eds.), Principle and practice in Applied Linguistics (pp. 125–144).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sylvén, L. K. (2006). How is extramural exposure to English among Swedish school
students used in the CLIL classroom? Vienna English Working Papers, 15(3),
47–53.
Zydatiß, W. (2012). Linguistic thresholds in the CLIL classrooms: The threshold
hypothesis revisited. International CLIL Research Journal, 1(4), 17–28.
Descargar