Subido por Juliana Gonzalez

Boersma Rebstock 09

Anuncio
MARINE ECOLOGY PROGRESS SERIES
Mar Ecol Prog Ser
Vol. 375: 263–275, 2009
doi: 10.3354/meps07753
Published January 27
Foraging distance affects reproductive success
in Magellanic penguins
P. Dee Boersma*, Ginger A. Rebstock
University of Washington, Department of Biology, Box 351800, Seattle, Washington 98195-1800, USA
ABSTRACT: Foraging distance affects reproductive success and other demographic parameters in
seabirds and pinnipeds. We tracked breeding Magellanic penguins Spheniscus magellanicus at
Punta Tombo, Argentina using satellite transmitters from 1996 to 2006 (n = 148 males, 57 females) to
investigate the variability in foraging distance and its effects on reproductive success. The time a
penguin was away from its nest predicted the distance it swam during all stages of the breeding season (p < 0.005); this relationship was linear during incubation and nonlinear during chick rearing.
During incubation, penguins went 1.0 km farther from the colony for every additional hour they were
away from the nest, and the distance penguins traveled predicted the mean colony reproductive success. When chicks were > 30 d old, the probability of fledging 2 chicks was highest when penguins
went less than 70 km from their nests. Penguins that went between 70 and 180 km from their nests
were most likely to raise one chick, and the probability of losing both chicks increased with trip distance. Females and males made trips of similar distance. Foraging-trip distance varied within breeding season (p < 0.005). Penguins went farthest during incubation (411 ± 11.8 km), and stayed closest
(61 ± 3.9 km) when chicks were < 30 d old, requiring parents to guard them. When chicks were older,
adults traveled 111 ± 5.0 km. Mean foraging-trip distance varied among years (p < 0.005) by factors
of 1.5 to 1.8, and trips by an individual penguin varied by a factor of 22.
KEY WORDS: Spheniscus magellanicus · Magellanic penguin · Foraging-trip distance · Variability ·
Reproductive success · Satellite tracking · Argos · Argentina
Resale or republication not permitted without written consent of the publisher
INTRODUCTION
Oceanographic conditions vary from year to year,
affecting the foraging locations and breeding success
of marine central-place foragers, such as seabirds and
pinnipeds, that feed at sea but breed on land (Boyd et
al. 1994, Kitaysky et al. 2000, Sydeman et al. 2001,
Davoren & Montevecchi 2003). Foraging locations at
sea, particularly distance to food, influence offspring
feeding frequency (Pinaud et al. 2005), reproductive
success (Inchausti et al. 2003), and adult energy balance (Shaffer et al. 2003). How central-place foragers
use the ocean affects not only their breeding parameters, but also their conservation, because of increasing
conflicts between human activities and wildlife
(Boersma 2008). Our knowledge of where seabirds and
pinnipeds forage has increased during the last decade
because of remote-tracking devices (e.g. Burger &
Shaffer 2008). In many cases, we know mean foragingtrip distances, but little about how distances and foraging areas vary. Quantifying the variability in trip distance among years is necessary for a variety of reasons
including understanding species’ energy needs, reproductive success, recruitment, population dynamics, and
determining ocean zoning and locations of protected
areas for management and conservation.
For central-place foragers, distance to foraging areas
is often related to trip duration and the frequency of
offspring feeding (Shaffer et al. 2003, Boersma et al.
2007). Many seabirds and pinnipeds feed at particular
oceanographic features (Hunt et al. 1999, Boersma et
al. in press), and flying or swimming from breeding
sites to the foraging areas make up a large percentage
of the trip duration. Boersma et al. (in press) found a
nonlinear relationship between trip distance and duration, with penguins spending more time in foraging
*Email: [email protected]
© Inter-Research 2009 · www.int-res.com
264
Mar Ecol Prog Ser 375: 263–275, 2009
areas farther from the colony. If foraging trips take
longer, offspring will be fed less frequently, resulting
in slower growth rates and lower weights at fledging or
weaning (Kitaysky et al. 2000, Davoren & Montevecchi
2003). Longer travel distances also require increased
energy expenditure that may affect adult body condition (Arnould et al. 1996) and the balance between
food assimilated by the adult and that delivered to offspring (Weimerskirch et al. 1994, Ropert-Coudert et al.
2004). In the case of seabirds that share incubation and
provisioning duties, long trip distances and durations
may also affect the fasting mate’s condition, eventually
causing abandonment of the nest (Yorio & Boersma
1994, Numata et al. 2000, Tveraa & Christensen 2002).
In some seabird species, males and females have different foraging strategies during the breeding season
(Clarke et al. 1998, González-Solís et al. 2008). This is
often linked to sexual size dimorphism, but also occurs
in species with males and females of similar size (Lewis
et al. 2002). Male Magellanic penguins are slightly
larger than females and are at sea a few weeks earlier
during incubation (Boersma et al. 1990). Either of these
differences may cause males and females to adopt different foraging strategies.
Trip distance often varies across the breeding season, reflecting different constraints (Weimerskirch et
al. 1993, Guinet et al. 1997, Beauplet et al. 2004,
Lescroël & Bost 2005, Phalan et al. 2007). Older chicks
have greater food needs and greater storage capacity
than younger chicks (Walker & Boersma 2003) which
may both require and allow adults to go farther to forage when chicks are older. Seabirds can go farthest
during incubation when there are no chicks to feed;
they need to stay closest to the colony when chicks are
small, have limited storage capacity (small stomachs
and limited energy reserves), and require the attendance of a parent for protection and thermoregulation.
Adults may go intermediate distances when chicks are
larger and have increased storage capacity and energy
needs. Additionally, prey distribution may change
during the breeding season, resulting in changes in
foraging-trip distance (Humphreys et al. 2006).
Interannual changes in prey distribution or availability may also cause foraging-trip distance to vary (Boyd
et al. 1994, Sydeman et al. 2001, Inchausti et al. 2003).
Longer foraging trips in years of low prey availability
may cause increased investment in reproduction by
adults (Boyd et al. 1994) and decreases in offspring
growth rates (Davoren & Montevecchi 2003, Pinaud et
al. 2005) and reproductive success (Inchausti et al.
2003). Climate fluctuations may affect species differently, depending on prey and foraging strategies
(Kitaysky et al. 2000).
We present foraging-trip data from 11 years of satellite tracking the at-sea locations of over 200 breeding
Magellanic penguins Spheniscus magellanicus at Punta
Tombo, Argentina. Using our large dataset we quantified the relationships between trip distance and duration, and between trip distance and reproductive success. We also tested whether foraging-trip distance
differed between males and females, and quantified
variability in trip distances within and among breeding
seasons.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Magellanic penguins breed at mainland and island
colonies in Argentina, Chile and the Falkland (Malvinas) Islands (Williams 1995). They eat primarily small
pelagic fish, hake Merluccius hubbsi, and squid
(Williams 1995) that are often associated with oceanographic frontal systems (Hansen et al. 2001, Acha et al.
2004). We divided breeding into 3 stages: (1) incubation, when mates alternate long incubation periods
with long-duration foraging trips, generally midOctober to late November; (2) early-chick rearing,
when mates alternate every couple of days between
attending the chicks and foraging, late November to
late December; and (3) late-chick rearing, when both
adults forage simultaneously and leave the chicks
unattended, from late December until fledging in January or February (Boersma et al. 1990). During incubation, males generally take their longest foraging trip
after egg laying, and females after the males return to
relieve them. Males are typically at sea during October
and females during November, with some overlap in
late October and early November. (Boersma et al.
1990). We checked nests daily until both chicks
hatched, and used chick age (< 30 d or > 30 d) to separate early-chick and late-chick period tracks. Year
refers to the calendar year of the beginning of the
breeding season, e.g. 2006 is the 2006–2007 season.
Satellite tracking. We tracked Magellanic penguins
at Punta Tombo, Argentina (44° 2.7’ S, 65° 13.4’ W)
using satellite transmitters and the Argos system
(Argos 2006). From the late-chick stage of 1996
through the 2006 season, we attached transmitters to
148 adult males and 57 adult females in a total of 268
deployments (Table 1). We tracked 49 penguins more
than once (43 males and 6 females) but in different
years. For 52 penguins transmitters did not start until
the penguin was far at sea, or failed prematurely,
resulting in incomplete trips that we did not use. We
attached the devices to feathers with fast-setting epoxy
or tape (Wilson & Wilson 1989) and epoxy. We reduced
drag on the devices by centering them low (6.6 ±
1.5 cm above the uropygial gland, n = 219) on the penguin’s back (Bannasch et al. 1994) and positioning
feathers over the leading edge.
265
Boersma & Rebstock: Foraging distances of Magellanic penguins
Table 1. Spheniscus magellanicus. Satellite transmitters deployed (n = 268) on Magellanic penguins at Punta Tombo from the
1996 breeding season through the 2006 season. Numbers in parentheses in ‘No. males’ and ‘No. females’ columns represent the
deployments (n = 216) with nearly complete trips, which were used to calculate trip distances. Inc = incubation; EC = early chick;
LC = late chick. See ‘Materials and methods’ for definitions. Deployment dates are dates transmitters were attached; transmitters
were left on for 30 to 90 d. Mean weight is at time of deployment
Year
Stage
Deployment
dates
No. males
Mean weight
± SE (kg)
No. females
1996
1997
LC
Inc
LC
Inc
EC
LC
Inc
EC
LC
Inc
EC
LC
Inc
EC
LC
Inc
LC
Inc
LC
Inc
LC
Inc
LC
Inc
LC
15 Dec 1996–18 Jan 1997
13–19 Oct 1997
23–31 Dec 1997
5–18 Oct 1998
8–17 Nov 1998
26 Dec 1998–12 Jan 1999
9–22 Oct 1999
22–27 Nov 1999
5–18 Jan 2000
7–28 Oct 2000
27 Nov–9 Dec 2000
5–13 Jan 2001
8–19 Oct 2001
13–19 Nov 2001
26–31 Dec 2001
15–25 Oct 2002
26 Dec 2002–2 Jan 2003
9–15 Oct 2003
21 Dec 2003–1 Jan 2004
3–10 Oct 2004
27 Dec 2004–8 Jan 2005
8–13 Oct 2005
26 Dec 2005–7 Jan 2006
9–20 Oct 2006
19–30 Dec 2006
5 (3)
3 (2)
5 (5)
4 (3)
6 (6)
6 (5)
5 (4)
5 (3)
4 (3)
17 (13)
6 (4)
7 (6)
6 (5)
4 (4)
8 (8)
12 (4)
12 (12)
10 (9)
7 (7)
12 (9)
13 (12)
12 (11)
12 (12)
12 (12)
12 (11)
4.44 ± 0.207
4.11 ± 0.176
4.89 ± 0.231
4.30 ± 0.120
4.44 ± 0.114
4.24 ± 0.159
4.04 ± 0.165
4.17 ± 0.199
4.33 ± 0.198
4.00 ± 0.110
3.88 ± 0.154
4.44 ± 0.118
4.08 ± 0.096
4.43 ± 0.138
4.47 ± 0.182
4.07 ± 0.130
4.33 ± 0.096
4.26 ± 0.070
4.44 ± 0.189
4.24 ± 0.069
4.38 ± 0.059
4.24 ± 0.091
4.53 ± 0.107
4.13 ± 0.071
4.74 ± 0.127
0
3 (3)
0
4 (2)
6 (6)
5 (5)
5 (1)
5 (3)
2 (2)
6 (0)
6 (5)
5 (4)
6 (2)
4 (4)
6 (6)
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
205 (173)
4.30 ± 0.028
63 (43)
1998
1999
2000
2001
2002
2003
2004
2005
2006
Totals
We used 3 types of transmitters (Table 1): ST-10s and
ST-20s transmitted every 45 s every day, Kiwisat transmitters were programmed to transmit every 60 s for
12 h out of each 48 h (12 h on : 36 h off). ST-20 and
Kiwisat transmitters were higher power (0.5 W) than
ST-10s (0.25 W) and provided more frequent and
higher-quality locations (authors’ unpubl. data).
Track processing. We used only Argos locations calculated from 4 or more messages received during each
satellite pass (location class 0-3). Argos estimates 1 SD of
errors in latitude and longitude of Class 1 to 3 locations to
be within 1000 m, while Class 0 locations have no estimate of error (Argos 2006). We applied 4 filters to the data
(authors unpubl.). The first filter removed locations closer
together than 45 min, to make the samples more evenly
spaced. The second and third filters were the speed filters
of Austin et al. (2003) and McConnell et al. (1992). The
4th filter removed locations that were far off the penguin’s track, but were not rejected by the speed filters because they were far apart in time (authors’ unpubl.).
We calculated trip distance as the straight-line distance from Punta Tombo to the farthest point of each
trip (Boersma et al. in press). This is less than the total
distance traveled by the penguins, because they often
Mean weight
± SE (kg)
3.76 ± 0.185
3.36 ± 0.063
4.41 ± 0.158
3.44 ± 0.070
3.13 ± 0.078
3.64 ± 0.173
3.80 ± 0.400
3.63 ± 0.162
3.47 ± 0.119
3.50 ± 0.085
3.27 ± 0.129
4.18 ± 0.165
3.49 ± 0.260
Transmitter
type(s)
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10 & Kiwisat
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-10
ST-20 & ST-10
ST-20 & ST-10
ST-20 & ST-10
ST-20
ST-20
ST-20
ST-20
3.61 ± 0.058
make looping trips with meanders. During incubation,
we used the longest trip recorded that began after the
first egg was laid for each bird. In 24 cases, we used
incomplete trips (those in which transmission began or
ended at sea but close to the colony relative to the trip
distance; 13.2 ± 12.6% mean ± SD) for distance calculations but not for estimating trip duration. During the
chick-rearing periods, we generally recorded more
than one trip per penguin. We defined a trip as at least
3 consecutive points that were at least 5 km from Punta
Tombo with at least one point preceding and following
within 5 km of Punta Tombo (Boersma et al. in press).
We used nest attendance data to determine when penguins returned to the colony, and if returns were not
recorded in the Argos data, we used the trips to calculate distance but not duration.
For each stage of the breeding season, we calculated
the maximum and mean trip distances for each penguin in each year. No penguin was tracked in more
than one period in the same year; hence we used one
value per individual except where the same penguin
was tracked in multiple years (n = 32). For incubation,
maximum and mean trip distances were the same
since we used one trip per penguin (per year).
266
Mar Ecol Prog Ser 375: 263–275, 2009
Foraging trip distance and duration. We previously
reported a positive relationship between trip distance
and duration for female penguins at Punta Tombo
(Boersma et al. 2007). Here we refined the relationships
using both males and females. The trip distances differed from those used in the earlier paper (Boersma et
al. 2007), because we used only Argos locations of Class
1 to 3 in the earlier regressions; in the present analysis
we also included Class 0 and filtered all 4 location
classes. Retention of Class-0 locations resulted in more
trips and sometimes longer trips in the current analysis.
Graphs of trip distance as a function of duration by
individual penguins during the chick-rearing stages
indicated that slopes and intercepts might differ by
individual. We fitted mixed-effects models (randomintercept models, allowing the intercept to vary among
penguins as in the previous study) and mixed-effects
random-coefficient models (allowing both intercept
and slope to vary among penguins) (Rabe-Hesketh &
Skrondal 2005). We included duration squared to test
for nonlinearity, since some curves appeared to flatten
out for longer trips. We included year, sex, and the
interaction between sex and duration as factors.
For incubation, we had a maximum of 3 trips (in different years) for any penguin. We calculated robust
SEs, with degrees of freedom equal to the number of
individual penguins, to account for the lack of independence when the same bird was tracked in more
than one year (Long & Freese 2006). For incubation, we
only used the higher-power ST-20 transmitters, deployed in 2004–2006 on males only, because we had
few complete incubation trips from ST-10 transmitters
and we could not estimate trip duration for the intermittent Kiwisat transmitters. For chick-rearing stages,
we ran regressions for the years 1998 to 2001, when we
tracked males and females, and all years for the latechick stage (1996 to 2006).
Foraging trip distance and reproductive success. We
measured reproductive success yearly in 2 areas within
the colony separated by about 320 m. We checked all
nests (mean ± SD = 173.5 ± 36.4 nests with eggs per
year) within the 2 study areas every 1 to 10 d from prior
to egg laying until fledging. We counted chicks as
fledged if they were last seen after 10 January, weighed
≥1800 g, and had not lost weight during the last few
measurements (Boersma et al. 1990). Reproductive success was calculated as the number of chicks fledged per
nest with eggs, with a maximum of 2. We tested the effect of mean distance from the colony that penguins
swam during incubation and the late-chick stage on
mean reproductive success using multiple linear regression. We did not test for an effect when chicks were
< 30 d old because we only had 4 years of data. We included precipitation as a covariate because heavy rain
can cause nests to flood or collapse, killing embryos and
small chicks (Boersma et al. 2004, Boersma 2008). We
measured rainfall daily with an on-site rain gauge, except in 2005, when we used data collected at the La
Regina Estancia, about 25 km from Punta Tombo. We
used total rainfall between 16 October and 15 December, when eggs and chicks are most vulnerable to
weather. The Estancia data for 2005 covered 1 October
to 31 December and although this was a longer period,
the total rainfall in 2005 was lower than what affected
reproductive success in the other years (Boersma 2008).
We removed the 3 years with heaviest rainfall from 16
October to 15 December (> 50 mm) and regressed reproductive success on mean trip distance during incubation and late-chick stages.
We also compared the number of chicks fledged by
each penguin that carried a transmitter during each
stage of the breeding season with the mean trip distance for that penguin using a multinomial logistic regression. We used only penguins that still had 2 chicks
when we attached the transmitters (28 of 33 penguins
when chicks were small and 79 of 100 penguins when
chicks were older). All years were combined and robust
SEs were calculated to account for the lack of independence in trips by the same penguin. Following the regression, we modeled the predicted probability of each
outcome for each mean distance (Long & Freese 2006).
We used an index based on the first principal component of bill, wing, and foot measurements (Yorio &
Boersma 1994, Hood et al. 1998) to evaluate the body
condition of 3 females that made unusually long trips
during late-chick stages of 1998 and 2001. Because of
small sample sizes in 1998 and 2001, we used all
females weighed and measured between 16 December
and 15 January 1983 to 2006 to develop the index (n =
473). We also compared an egg-volume index (length ×
width2) for 2 of the females to the means for 1998 (n =
538 1st eggs, 508 2nd eggs), as lower clutch volumes
reflect poor female body condition at egg laying
(Moreno et al. 2002). Egg size has little influence on
nesting success but it does reflect parental quality in
Magellanic penguins (Reid & Boersma 1990).
Variation between sexes, within and among seasons. We used multiple regression to test for effects of
stage of the breeding season, year, and sex on trip distances. We included year as a categorical factor rather
than a continuous variable because we expected interannual differences in prey availability but not necessarily a linear trend. In addition to regressions for 1996
to 2006, we ran separate tests for 1997 to 2001, the only
years in which we tracked females (Table 1). We also
ran regressions for 1998 to 2001, the only years in
which we tracked during the early-chick stage (both
sexes). We also included start date of incubation trip
instead of sex in one set of regressions because date
may be more important than sex, and variability in trip
Boersma & Rebstock: Foraging distances of Magellanic penguins
timing within males and females may mask the effects
of date.
Regression models were run in Stata 9.2 (StataCorp
LP). Examination of the residuals did not indicate
problems with any of the regression fits. Means are
presented ± 1 SE.
RESULTS
Foraging-trip distance and duration
Trip distance was positively correlated with trip duration, although the details of the relationships varied
over the breeding season (Table 2). Trip distance depended linearly on duration during incubation (26 trips
by 23 penguins, males only, F3,22 = 23.65, p < 0.005, R2 =
0.74; coefficient for duration: t = 8.01, p < 0.005) but not
on year (p = 0.41 and 0.75). Duration squared was not
significant (p = 0.84), indicating the relationship was
linear. Penguins went 1.0 ± 0.13 km farther from the
colony for every hour they were away from the nest.
Distance depended nonlinearly on duration when
penguins had small chicks (152 trips by 33 penguins):
both duration (Z = 16.0, p < 0.005) and duration squared
(Z = –7.19, p < 0.005) were significant (Table 2). The
relationship between distance and duration also varied
significantly with year (p < 0.005): slopes were steeper
in 2000 and 2001 when penguins went farther from the
colony than in 1998 and 1999 when they stayed closer
to their nests. Penguins generally swam farther in 2000
and 2001 than in the first 2 years, but trip durations
were not proportionately longer. Sex (Z = –1.27, p =
0.20) and the interaction between sex and duration
(Z = 1.28, p = 0.20) were not significant. Trips by different penguins had similar slopes: the random-coefficient model was not significantly better than the random-intercept model (likelihood-ratio test: LR χ2(2) =
1.82, p = 0.4). Moreover, correcting for the conservative nature of the likelihood-ratio test (Rabe-Hesketh &
Skrondal 2005) gave similar results. Results of a linear
Table 2. Spheniscus magellanicus. Relationship between foraging-trip distance and duration during 3 stages of the breeding season. Relationships were nonlinear if duration squared
was included in the best model. Only males were used in the
regression for incubation. Slopes by individual penguin could
not be tested for incubation because we used only 1 trip per
penguin (per year). N/A: not applicable
Linear
Years differ
Sexes differ
Slopes differ
Incubation
Early chick
Late chick
Yes
No
N/A
N/A
No
Yes
No
No
No
No
No
Yes
267
regression run without duration squared were similar
to those of Boersma et al. (2007), after excluding a
female that went almost 300 km after her chicks died:
in both analyses the slope was 0.87 ± 0.07.
The non-linear relationship between distance and duration of penguin foraging trips was also apparent when
penguins had large chicks (191 trips by 37 penguins):
duration (Z = 15.84, p < 0.005) and duration squared (Z =
–7.36, p < 0.005) were significant (Table 2). The relationship between trip distance and duration did not depend
on year (p = 0.15 to 0.70), sex (p = 0.93), or the interaction
between sex and duration (p = 0.69). However, individual penguins had different slopes (LR χ2(2) = 61.33, p <
0.005). Results from a linear regression were similar to
those reported in Boersma et al. (2007) for females: in
both cases, penguins went 0.87 ± 0.04 km farther from
the colony for each hour they were gone.
Foraging-trip distance and reproductive success
As foraging-trip distance increased, mean reproductive success of the colony decreased. Rainfall between
16 October and 15 December negatively affected
reproductive success (F1,8 = 33.1, p = 0.0004, adjusted
R2 = 0.78, RMSE = 0.136) (Fig. 1). Adding mean trip distance during incubation as a covariate improved the
fit (F2,7 = 22.96, p = 0.0008, adjusted R2 = 0.83, RMSE =
0.120). However, adding mean trip distance during
late chick did not improve the fit (rain only: adjusted
R2 = 0.77, RMSE = 0.147; rain + trip distance: adjusted
R2 = 0.75, RMSE = 0.152). With the 3 years of heaviest
rainfall removed, reproductive success depended on
Fig. 1. Spheniscus magellanicus. Mean reproductive success
(number of chicks fledged per nest with eggs) in 2 areas of the
colony as a function of the mean distance penguins traveled
from the colony during incubation. Points are labeled with
year and total rainfall (parentheses, in mm) between 16 October and 15 December. (d) years with < 50 mm rain; (m) > 50 mm
rain; (Line) least-squares regression excluding 3 years with
> 50 mm rain (reproductive success = 1.5 – 0.002 × mean distance; adjusted R2 = 0.51)
268
Mar Ecol Prog Ser 375: 263–275, 2009
trip distance during incubation (F1,5 = 7.28, p = 0.04,
adjusted R2 = 0.51, n = 7) (Fig. 1), but not during late
chick (F1,6 < 0.1, p = 0.96, n = 8).
Foraging-trip distance also predicted individuals’
reproductive success (Fig. 2). The multinomial logistic
regression of number of chicks fledged on mean trip
distance for transmitter-equipped birds with chicks
older than 30 d was significant (Wald χ2 = 5.96, p =
0.015). The probability of fledging 2 chicks was high
(> 0.5) for penguins that made the shortest trips
(< 70 km) and decreased with increasing distance to
< 0.01 (Fig. 2). The probability of fledging zero chicks
was low (~0.01) for birds making very short trips but
increased with increasing distance to ~0.8. The probability of fledging one chick peaked at intermediate foraging distances (~70 to 180 km) and was lower at short
and long distances. There was no relationship between
number of chicks fledged and mean trip distance during incubation (Wald χ2 = 0.69, p = 0.71) or when chicks
were small (Wald χ2 = 1.45, p = 0.48).
Variation between sexes
Males and females traveled similar distances during
incubation (all years and 1997 to 2001: p ≥ 0.46)
(Fig. 3). Foraging-trip distance was not related to trip
start date (p ≥ 0.36), suggesting the prey distribution
was similar throughout incubation. When chicks were
small, males and females also traveled similar distances (mean and maximum distance: p ≥ 0.67). When
chicks were > 30 d old, maximum but not mean distance was greater for females than for males (all years
Fig. 2. Spheniscus magellanicus. Predicted probability of
Magellanic penguin fledging success (0, 1, or 2 chicks) calculated from the multinomial regression of chicks fledged
against mean foraging-trip distance during the late-chick
stage (n = 78). Predicted fledging probabilities are plotted
against actual mean distances. One female that traveled
> 300 km was excluded. Each penguin tracked had 2 chicks
30 d of age or older when the transmitters were attached in
late December or early January 1996–2007
Fig. 3. Spheniscus magellanicus. Mean trip distance by stage
of the breeding season, year, and sex for Magellanic penguins
breeding at Punta Tombo, Argentina. Error bars are 1 SE. See
Table 1 for sample sizes. (s) incubation; (h) early chick;
(n) late chick; (
) males; (----) females. Distance varied
by stage and year, but not sex
and 1998 to 2001, maximum distance: p = 0.03 and
0.04; mean distance: p ≥ 0.21).
The difference between the sexes in maximum foraging-trip distance during late chick was driven by 3 females (2 in 1998, 1 in 2001) that took trips of 275, 307,
and 372 km with durations of 23, 19.5, and 18.2 d,
respectively (see Fig. 6f). Excluding these 3 trips there
were no significant differences between the sexes, nor
was the interaction between sex and breeding stage
significant (Table 3). The mean trip duration for late
chick, excluding the 3 females, was 2.9 d; the 3 females
made trips 6 times the mean duration. The longest trip
by any other penguin was 10.8 d. The female that made
an unusually long trip in 2001 of 18.2 d had the lowest
body condition index in our dataset (n = 473). The 2
females that made unusually long trips in 1998 were
also in poor condition (negative index), but did not have
lower index values than other females we tracked.
However, they both laid at least one small egg
(1st eggs: 219.9 and 232.1 cm3; 2nd eggs: 213.0 and
218.0 cm3) compared to the mean egg size for 1998
(1st eggs: 231.7 ± 0.9 cm3, n = 538; 2nd eggs: 228.8 ±
1.0 cm3, n = 506), indicating that they were in poor body
condition when they laid eggs. All 3 females returned
to their nests before the end of the season, and 2 of the 3
bred in subsequent years. Males were seen at the nests
2 to 15 times while the females were at sea and were
able to compensate in part for the females’ absence, as
2 of the 3 pairs fledged one chick. The third pair had
one chick that was still alive but weighed <1.8 kg (our
criterion for fledging) when last seen on 3 February,
> 2 wk after the return of the female. A few males made
trips > 275 km from the colony, but none made trips
with duration >11 d during chick rearing.
Boersma & Rebstock: Foraging distances of Magellanic penguins
Table 3. Spheniscus magellanicus. Multiple regressions of
maximum and mean trip distance on stage of the breeding
season, year (as a categorical variable), sex, and the interaction between sex and stage. Foraging-trip distance depended
on stage of the breeding season and year. The significant
coefficients for sex (for maximum distance only) and the interaction between sex and stage were driven by 3 females that
made anomalous trips (see ‘Results’). Statistics for sex and the
interaction excluding those 3 females are also shown. Inc: incubation; LC: late chick; EC: early chick
p
R2
Maximum distance
44.215,171
Inc (vs. LC)
15.33
EC (vs. LC)
–5.36
Year (1996–1999 and
3.73–0.06
2001–2006 vs. 2000)
Sex
1.94
Sex, excluding 3 females
0.80
Sex × Stage
–2.15
Sex × Stage, excluding
–1.57
3 females
< 0.005
< 0.005
< 0.005
< 0.005–0.95
0.79
Mean distance
52.915,171
Inc (vs. LC)
19.74
EC (vs. LC)
–3.21
Year (1996–1999 and
2.77–0.05
2001–2006 vs. 2000)
Sex
1.38
Sex, excluding 3 females
–0.51
Sex × Stage
–2.05
Sex × Stage, excluding
–1.31
3 females
< 0.005
< 0.005
0.002
0.006–0.96
Response variable
or predictor
variable
F for
regression or t for
predictor variables
0.05
0.42
0.03
0.12
0.84
0.17
0.61
0.04
0.19
Variation within the breeding season
Penguins traveled farthest during incubation (411 ±
11.8 km, n = 80) (Table 3, Figs. 3 & 4), and stayed closest to the colony when chicks were small (mean trip
distance = 61 ± 3.9 km, mean maximum trip distance =
92 ± 6.5 km, n = 35) (Fig. 5). During the late-chick
period when chicks had a larger storage capacity, distances were intermediate (mean trip distance = 111 ±
5.0 km, mean maximum trip distance = 176 ± 5.9 km,
n = 101) (Fig. 6). Distances among all 3 stages of the
breeding season were significantly different (Table 3).
The direction penguins swam from the colony, generally northeast, varied little among stages or years
(Figs. 4–6). No penguins used the extensive areas
south or southeast of the colony when breeding.
Variation among years
Trip distances during incubation increased from
1997 through 2002, and then declined, though not to
the level of the earlier years (Table 3, Fig. 3). In the first
269
5 yr of tracking (1997 to 2001), 3 of 35 penguins (8.6%)
traveled more than 500 km (Fig. 4a–c). In the last 5 yr
(2002 to 2006), 13 of 45 penguins (28.9%) traveled
> 500 km (G = 5.49, p = 0.02) (Fig. 4d –f). The mean trip
distance for 1997 to 2001 was 386 ± 18.7 km, and in
2002 to 2006 the mean trip distance increased to 431 ±
14.7 km (t69 = –1.86, p = 0.07). In 2002, all penguins
swam > 500 km from the colony (n = 4) (Fig. 4d). Two of
the males that we tracked in 2002 took the first incubation shift, and therefore did not leave their nests when
the transmitters were attached. Males rarely incubate
first but will when females are in poor body condition
(Yorio & Boersma 1994).
Distances to foraging locations during chick rearing
were variable among years but overall increased with
year (Figs. 3, 5 & 6). When chicks were small, both
mean (simple linear regression: F1,32 = 10.83, p = 0.002,
R2 = 0.13) and maximum distances (F1,32 = 9.55, p =
0.004, R2 = 0.17) increased over 4 yr (Figs. 3 & 5). When
chicks were older, maximum distances (simple linear
regression: F1,84 = 5.98, p = 0.02, R2 = 0.07), but not
mean distances (F1,84 = 2.37, p = 0.13) increased over
11 yr (Figs. 3 & 6).
The annual mean maximum distances during incubation, early-chick, and late-chick stages varied by
factors of approximately 1.8, 1.6 and 1.9, respectively. Annual mean trip distances during early-chick
and late-chick stages varied by factors of 1.5 and 1.7,
respectively. However, individual trips varied more,
with trip distances ranging from 94 to 605 km during
incubation, 6 to 182 km during early chick, and 7 to
372 km during late chick (Figs. 4–6). One penguin
made trips ranging from 8 to 182 km during early
chick and another made trips ranging from 17 to
289 km during late chick. During incubation, the
mean difference between the shortest and longest
trips within years was 253 ± 35.1 km (Fig. 4). The
shortest difference in trip distance during incubation
within a year was 69 km in 2002; the longest was
456 km in 2000. When chicks were small, the mean
difference among trips within years was 137 ±
19.2 km, and when chicks were large, 241 ± 21.5 km
(Figs. 5 & 6).
DISCUSSION
Foraging-trip distance varied within and among
years, as well as among penguins and among trips by
the same penguin (Figs. 3–6). Magellanic penguins
feed along fronts and in areas of high productivity
(Boersma et al. in press). Many seabirds forage primarily in productive oceanographic features (Hunt et al.
1999), but some variability among years is probably
the rule, as oceanographic features vary in strength
270
Mar Ecol Prog Ser 375: 263–275, 2009
Fig. 4. Spheniscus magellanicus.
Foraging trip tracks for Magellanic penguins breeding at Punta
Tombo, Argentina, during incubation. (A) red = 1997, (B) black =
1998, red = 1999, (C) black =
2000, red = 2001, (D) black =
2002, red = 2003, (E) black = 2004,
red = 2005, (F) black = 2006
from year to year and those that are not fixed bathymetrically move. Differences in foraging-trip distance
or duration within species among years are common in
multi-year studies and likely reflect changes in prey
availability (e.g. Kitaysky et al. 2000, Ballard et al.
2001, Boersma et al. 2002, Lescroël & Bost 2005). Vari-
ability among individuals from the same colony is
probably common as well (Ballard et al. 2001), reflecting factors such as age, experience, body condition,
and number and size of chicks. Variability in foraging
trips among individuals and among years is seldom
accounted for in modeling studies (e.g. Green et al.
Boersma & Rebstock: Foraging distances of Magellanic penguins
271
Fig. 5. Spheniscus magellanicus. Foraging trip
tracks for Magellanic
penguins breeding at
Punta Tombo, Argentina, during early chick
(chicks < 30 d old). (A)
black = 1998, red =
1999, (B) black = 2000,
red = 2001
2007), but it is large and affects adult and offspring
energy balances as well as demographic parameters
such as reproductive success and recruitment. It is also
an important consideration for management and conservation. Marine protected areas and ocean zoning
must take variability in trip distance into account to
protect penguins in all their foraging areas, particularly when human and penguin uses of an area conflict.
Foraging-trip distance and duration
A positive relationship between trip distance and
duration is known for several penguin species (e.g.
Petersen et al. 2006, Boersma et al. 2007), but how
that relationship varies is unknown. Our large dataset
allowed us to refine the relationship for Magellanic
penguins, and determine that it is linear during incubation and nonlinear during chick rearing. When
chicks are older the relationship varies by individual
penguin. Variation among penguins likely reflects
luck as well as the foraging ability and experience of
the individual and leads to variation in the reproductive outcome of mating pairs. The simple linear
relationship between distance and duration was
robust, suggesting that (1) duration is a good predictor
of how far penguins travel, and (2) choice of filtering
methods, at least for some analyses, may make little
difference.
Penguins that traveled the farthest from the colony
stayed away longer than predicted by a linear relationship. Boersma et al. (in press) found that penguins that
go farther from the colony remain in the foraging area
longer, probably because they need to replenish the
extra energy required to swim greater distances. Our
findings are consistent with this hypothesis: additional
time spent in the foraging area on longer trips increases trip duration but not trip distance.
Foraging-trip distance and reproductive success
Trip distance influenced reproductive success at
2 levels. At the colony level, the mean distance
penguins with satellite transmitters traveled during incubation each year was reflected in mean annual reproductive success. If a penguin is away from the nest
too long during incubation, the incubating mate may
abandon the eggs to forage for itself, or the chicks may
hatch and starve before the foraging parent returns
with food (Yorio & Boersma 1994, Boersma & Stokes
1995, Otley et al. 2004). During chick rearing, longer
trips mean less frequent feeding of chicks, slower
growth, and higher risk of starvation or low fledging
weights (Davoren & Montevecchi 2003). The major
cause of mortality for Magellanic penguin chicks is
starvation (Boersma & Stokes 1995), and lone chicks
grow more rapidly than chicks with siblings, suggesting that food is limited for Magellanic penguins
(Boersma 1992). We did not detect a significant relationship between mean trip distance during late chick
and mean colony reproductive success, perhaps because the penguins we tracked in that stage were not
representative. We selected only penguins that had
healthy chicks > 30 d old for tracking in late chick.
However, the mean reproductive success in the colony
included many penguins that had already lost both
eggs or chicks by that time. The penguins we selected
to track during late chick may have been better foragers than average, since many chicks starve before
reaching 30 d (Boersma & Stokes 1995). Reproductive
272
Mar Ecol Prog Ser 375: 263–275, 2009
Fig. 6. Spheniscus magellanicus. Foraging trip tracks for Magellanic penguins breeding at Punta Tombo, Argentina, during late chick (chicks > 30 d
old). (A) black = 1996, red = 1997,
(B) black = 1998, red = 1999, (C) black =
2000, red = 2001, (D) black = 2002, red
= 2003, (E) black = 2004, red = 2005,
(F) black = 2006, blue = 3 females that
made trips of unusually long duration
in 1998 and 2001
success approached one chick per nest with eggs in
only one of 25 years in our long-term study of this
colony, and average reproductive success is usually less
than half a chick per nest with eggs (Boersma 2008).
The number of chicks fledged was negatively related
to an individual’s mean trip distance during late chick
(Fig. 2). Penguins that swim too far during foraging
trips may not be able to deliver meals frequently
enough to keep a chick alive, however, those that find
food very close to the colony (< 70 km) may fledge both
chicks. Penguins that travel intermediate distances (70
to 180 km) may be able to keep one chick alive, but not
Boersma & Rebstock: Foraging distances of Magellanic penguins
both. Magellanic penguins feed the larger chick more
than the smaller chick (Boersma 1992), so the smaller
chick may not get enough food to keep it alive until the
next feeding. These data show that the average distance a penguin forages for its chicks is an important
determinant of reproductive success.
During incubation and early chick there was no relationship between the number of chicks fledged and
trip distance. Many eggs and young chicks were lost to
predators, obscuring the relationship between trip distance and reproductive success. In addition, if chicks
fledged at lighter weights when trips were longer, trip
distance could have an effect on future recruitment
as well as current reproduction (Mougin et al. 2000,
McClung et al. 2004).
273
(Sánchez & de Ciechomski 1995). Magellanic penguin
adults in good condition can fast for at least a month,
allowing the foraging mate to reach these aggregations during incubation and recover weight lost during
settlement, courtship, and incubation (Boersma et al.
1990). Chlorophyll a concentrations peak in November
in coastal waters on the Patagonian shelf at 43° S (Rivas
et al. 2006). By November and December, when chicks
hatch and need to be fed frequently, coastal fronts
have formed (Rivas et al. 2006) and anchovy aggregations are near Punta Tombo (Sánchez & de Ciechomski
1995), allowing penguins to make much shorter trips
during early chick than during incubation.
Variation among years
Variation between sexes
We expected males to make longer trips than
females during incubation, when males are at sea earlier than females. Seasonal coastal fronts form (Rivas et
al. 2006) and spawning schools of anchovy Engraulis
anchoita move south (Sánchez & de Ciechomski 1995)
during this period. Hence prey may be more abundant
or available farther north when males make their long
incubation foraging trips and farther south when
females make their trips. However, we found no evidence that females and males consistently swam different distances from the colony during any stage of
the breeding season. Three females with chicks > 30 d
old were away from their nests for 18 to 23 d, longer
than any males. We tracked about 5 times more males
(84) than females (17) during late chick, but only
females made these long trips. We were unable to tell
if these females had abandoned their breeding
attempts, but all 3 returned to their nests and their
chicks were still alive when they returned. These long
trips likely serve to replenish body condition and may
be particularly important for females. Females are
smaller than males, have a more limited storage capacity, and a less favorable surface-to-volume ratio, and
hence are more likely to be in poor condition after their
fast (see below) than males (Hood et al. 1998). Magellanic penguins are opportunistic foragers and prey on
whatever they can find. Thus, individual differences as
we found are likely more important than differences
between the sexes.
Variation within the breeding season
Spawning aggregations of anchovy, a primary prey
of Magellanic penguins (Frere et al. 1996, Wilson et al.
2005), are found north of Punta Tombo in October
Variability in trip distance among years likely reflects environmental conditions and prey availability.
Magellanic penguin foraging locations depend on
local productivity and oceanographic fronts (Boersma
et al. in press), so trip distances may reflect prey distribution and abundance. Magellanic penguins eat
highly mobile pelagic prey such as anchovy that
aggregate in fronts. Strength and location of fronts,
and associated anchovy distributions, vary among
years (Hansen et al. 2001), therefore penguin foragingtrip distances vary as well.
Penguins went farther from the colony during incubation in 2002 than in any other year. Foraging-trip
distances were less variable in 2002 than in other
years, in spite of having the highest mean; no penguins
were able to find food close to the colony. Food was
apparently scarce in 2002, as fewer penguins returned
to the colony to breed, egg laying was delayed, and
penguins that did breed were in poorer body condition
than usual (P.D.B. unpubl. data). These factors, along
with the unusual situation of males taking the first
incubation shifts, indicated low food availability both
prior to the breeding season and during incubation.
However, during late chick 2002, penguins did not
swim especially far compared to other years, indicating
either that conditions had improved by January, or that
penguins that still had chicks in January were very
good at finding food.
Toxic algal blooms sometimes kill penguins (Shumway et al. 2003) and may also have affected the penguins in 2002. Four penguins carrying tags did not
return from their incubation trips and were not seen
again, suggesting they died, which is rare during the
breeding season. It is possible they starved, but it
seems unlikely because, in general, penguins return
after their first incubation foraging trip in good condition. In 2000, 9 breeding penguins that we tracked
appeared healthy right before they left on foraging
Mar Ecol Prog Ser 375: 263–275, 2009
274
Antarctic fur seals. J Zool Lond 239:769–782
trips; these penguins failed to return and were not seen
again, coincident with a toxic algal bloom and large ➤ Austin D, McMillan JI, Bowen WD (2003) A three-stage algorithm for filtering erroneous Argos satellite locations. Mar
seabird mortality event in the region (Shumway et al.
Mamm Sci 19:371–383
2003). Besides the 9 penguins in 2000 and 4 in 2002,
➤ Ballard G, Ainley D, Ribic C, Barton K (2001) Effect of instruonly 3 other tracked penguins failed to return from
ment attachment and other factors on foraging trip duration and nesting success of Adelie penguins. Condor 103:
incubation trips, 2 in 1999 and one in 2001.
481–490
Since 2002, significantly more penguins traveled
Bannasch R, Wilson RP, Culick B (1994) Hydrodynamic
farther during incubation than in 1997 to 2001, sugaspects of design and attachment of a back-mounted
gesting changes in prey availability. In addition, trip
device in penguins. J Exp Biol 194:83–96
distances during early chick increased linearly over ➤ Beauplet G, Dubroca L, Guinet C, Cherel Y, Dabin W, Gagne
C, Hindell M (2004) Foraging ecology of subantarctic fur
4 yr, and mean maximum trip distance increased over
seals Arctocephalus tropicalis breeding on Amsterdam
11 yr during late chick. These longer distances may
Island: seasonal changes in relation to maternal characterindicate a longer-term shift in prey distribution or
istics and pup growth. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 273:211–225
availability reflecting climate variation.
Boersma PD (1992) Asynchronous hatching and food alloca-
CONCLUSIONS
➤
Foraging distance is closely related to the time a
penguin is away from the nest and is an important
determinant of reproductive success. Understanding
the variability in animal use of the marine environment
is critical for effective management, conservation, and
reserve design. Marine reserves or zones need to be
large or flexible enough to meet the needs of the species of interest throughout the annual cycle and among
years with different prey availability. Our data show
that the ocean habitat requirements of Magellanic
penguins are large and variable. Management of the ➤
South Atlantic ecosystem should take this variability
into account to conserve penguins as well as other
➤
seabirds and marine mammals in the area.
➤
Acknowledgements. The Wildlife Conservation Society,
ExxonMobil Foundation, Esso Argentina, National Geographic Society, Thorne Foundation, Offield Foundation, Disney Wildlife Conservation Fund, MKCG Foundation, The
Pew Charitable Trust marine fellows program, Friends of the
Penguins and the Wadsworth Endowed Chair in Conservation Science supported the project. The research was carried
out under a joint agreement between the Wildlife Conservation Society and the Office of Tourism, Province of Chubut,
Argentina. We thank the Province of Chubut and the La
Regina family for access to the penguin colony, and the many
students and field volunteers who collected data over the
years. S. Moore, E. Skewgar, A. Van Buren, and 3 anonymous
reviewers improved the manuscript.
LITERATURE CITED
➤ Acha
EM, Mianzan HW, Guerrero RA, Favero M, Bava J
(2004) Marine fronts at the continental shelves of austral
South America: physical and ecological processes. J Mar
Syst 44:83–105
Argos (2006) Argos user’s manual. CLS, Ramonville SaintAgne, available at www.argos-system.org/manual/
Arnould JPY, Boyd IL, Speakman JR (1996) The relationship
between foraging behaviour and energy expenditure in
➤
➤
tion in the Magellanic penguin Spheniscus magellanicus.
In: Bell BD (ed) Acta XX Congressus Internationalis
Ornithologici Wellington, NZ. Ornithological Congress
Trust Board, Christchurch, p 961–973
Boersma PD (2008) Penguins as marine sentinels. Bioscience
58:597–607
Boersma PD, Stokes DL (1995) Mortality patterns, hatching
asynchrony, and size asymmetry in Magellanic penguin
(Spheniscus magellanicus) chicks. In: Dann P, Norman I,
Reilly P (eds) The penguins: ecology and management.
Surrey Beatty and Sons, Chipping Norton, New South
Wales, p 3–25
Boersma PD, Stokes DL, Yorio PM (1990) Reproductive variability and historical change of Magellanic penguins
(Spheniscus magellanicus) at Punta Tombo, Argentina. In:
Davis L, Darby J (eds) Penguin biology. Academic Press,
San Diego, CA, p 13–43
Boersma PD, Stokes DL, Strange IJ (2002) Applying ecology
to conservation: tracking breeding penguins at New Island
South Reserve, Falkland Islands. Aquat Conserv 12:63–74
Boersma PD, Rebstock GA, Stokes DL (2004) Why penguin
eggshells are thick. Auk 121:148–155
Boersma PD, Rebstock GA, Stokes DL, Majluf P (2007)
Oceans apart: conservation models for two temperate penguin species shaped by the marine environment. Mar Ecol
Prog Ser 335:217–225
Boersma PD, Rebstock GA, Frere E, Moore SE (in press) Following the fish: penguins and productivity in the South
Atlantic. Ecol Monogr
Boyd IL, Arnould JPY, Barton T, Croxall JP (1994) Foraging
behaviour of Antarctic fur seals during periods of contrasting prey abundance. J Anim Ecol 63:703–713
Burger AE, Shaffer SA (2008) Application of tracking and
data-logging technology in research and conservation of
seabirds. Auk 125:253–264
Clarke J, Manly B, Kerry K, Gardner H, Franchi E, Corsolini
S, Focardi S (1998) Sex differences in Adélie penguin
foraging strategies. Polar Biol 20:248–258
Davoren GK, Montevecchi WA (2003) Consequences of foraging trip duration on provisioning behaviour and fledging
condition of common murres Uria aalge. J Avian Biol 34:
44–53
Frere E, Gandini P, Lichtschein V (1996) Variacion latitudinal
en la dieta del pinguino de Magallanes (Spheniscus
magellanicus) en la costa Patagonica, Argentina. Ornitol
Neotrop 7:35–41
González-Solís J, Croxall JP, Afanasyev V (2008) Offshore
spatial segregation in giant petrels Macronectes spp.:
differences between species, sexes and seasons. Aquat
Conserv 17:S22–S36
Boersma & Rebstock: Foraging distances of Magellanic penguins
➤ Green
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
JA, Boyd IL, Woakes AJ, Green CJ, Butler PJ (2007)
Feeding, fasting and foraging success during chick
rearing in macaroni penguins. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 346:
299–312
Guinet C, Koudil M, Bost CA, Durbec JP, Georges JY, Mouchot MC, Jouventin P (1997) Foraging behaviour of satellite-tracked king penguins in relation to sea-surface
temperatures obtained by satellite telemetry at Crozet
Archipelago, a study during three austral summers. Mar
Ecol Prog Ser 150:11–20
Hansen JE, Martos P, Madirolas A (2001) Relationship
between spatial distribution of the Patagonian stock of
Argentine anchovy, Engraulis anchoita, and sea temperatures during late spring to early summer. Fish Oceanogr
10:193–206
Hood LC, Boersma PD, Wingfield JC (1998) The adrenocortical response to stress in incubating Magellanic penguins
(Spheniscus magellanicus). Auk 115:76–84
Humphreys EM, Wanless S, Bryant DM (2006) Stage-dependent foraging in breeding black-legged kittiwakes Rissa
tridactyla: distinguishing behavioural responses to intrinsic and extrinsic factors. J Avian Biol 37:436–446
Hunt GL Jr, Mehlum F, Russell RW, Irons D, Decker MB,
Becker PH (1999) Physical processes, prey abundance,
and the foraging ecology of seabirds. In: Adams NJ,
Slotow R (eds) 22nd International Ornithological Congress. BirdLife South Africa, Johannesburg, p 2040–2056
Inchausti P, Guinet C, Koudil M, Durbec JP and others (2003)
Inter-annual variability in the breeding performance of
seabirds in relation to oceanographic anomalies that affect
the Crozet and the Kerguelen sectors of the Southern
Ocean. J Avian Biol 34:170–176
Kitaysky AS, Hunt GL Jr, Flint EN, Rubega MA, Decker MB
(2000) Resource allocation in breeding seabirds: responses
to fluctuations in their food supply. Mar Ecol Prog Ser
206:283–296
Lescroël A, Bost CA (2005) Foraging under contrasting
oceanographic conditions: the gentoo penguin at Kerguelen Archipelago. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 302:245–261
Lewis S, Benvenuti S, Dall’Antonia L, Griffiths R and others
(2002) Sex-specific foraging behaviour in a monomorphic
seabird. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:1687–1693
Long JS, Freese J (2006) Regression models for categorical
dependent variables using Stata. Stata Press, College
Station, TX
McClung MR, Seddon PJ, Massaro M, Setiawan AN (2004)
Nature-based tourism impacts on yellow-eyed penguins
Megadyptes antipodes: Does unregulated visitor access
affect fledging weight and juvenile survival? Biol Conserv
119:279–285
McConnell BJ, Chambers C, Fedak MA (1992) Foraging ecology of southern elephant seals in relation to the bathymetry and productivity of the Southern Ocean. Antarct Sci
4:393–398
Moreno J, Yorio P, Garcia-Borboroglu P, Potti J, Villar S (2002)
Health state and reproductive output in Magellanic
penguins (Spheniscus magellanicus). Ethol Ecol Evol 14:
19–28
Mougin JL, Jouanin C, Roux F, Zino F (2000) Fledging weight
and juvenile survival of Cory’s shearwaters Calonectris
diomedea on Selvagem Grande. Ringing Migr 20:107–110
Numata M, Davis LS, Renner M (2000) Prolonged foraging
trips and egg desertion in little penguins (Eudyptula
minor). NZ J Zool 27:277–289
Editorial responsibility: Charles Peterson,
Morehead City, North Carolina, USA
275
➤ Otley HM, Clausen AP, Christie DJ, Pütz K (2004) Aspects of
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
the breeding biology of the Magellanic penguin in the
Falkland Islands. Waterbirds 27:396–405
Petersen SL, Ryan PG, Gremillet D (2006) Is food availability
limiting African penguins Spheniscus demersus at Boulders? A comparison of foraging effort at mainland and
island colonies. Ibis 148:14–26
Phalan B, Phillips RA, Silk JRD, Afanasyev V and others
(2007) Foraging behaviour of four albatross species by
night and day. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 340:271–286
Pinaud D, Cherel Y, Weimerskirch H (2005) Effect of environmental variability on habitat selection, diet, provisioning
behaviour and chick growth in yellow-nosed albatrosses.
Mar Ecol Prog Ser 298:295–304
Rabe-Hesketh S, Skrondal A (2005) Multilevel and longitudinal modeling using Stata. Stata Press, College Station, TX
Reid WV, Boersma PD (1990) Parental quality and selection
on egg size in the Magellanic penguin. Evolution 44:
1780–1786
Rivas AL, Dogliotti AI, Gagliardini DA (2006) Seasonal variability in satellite-measured surface chlorophyll in the
Patagonian Shelf. Cont Shelf Res 26:703–720
Ropert-Coudert Y, Wilson RP, Daunt F, Kato A (2004) Patterns
of energy acquisition by a central place forager: benefits of
alternating short and long foraging trips. Behav Ecol 15:
824–830
Sánchez RP, de Ciechomski JD (1995) Spawning and nursery
grounds of pelagic fish species in the sea-shelf off
Argentina and adjacent areas. Sci Mar 59:455–478
Shaffer SA, Costa DP, Weimerskirch H (2003) Foraging effort
in relation to the constraints of reproduction in freeranging albatrosses. Funct Ecol 17:66–74
Shumway SE, Allen SM, Boersma PD (2003) Marine birds and
harmful algal blooms: sporadic victims or under-reported
events? Harmful Algae 2:1–17
Sydeman WJ, Hester MM, Thayer JA, Gress F, Martin P,
Buffa J (2001) Climate change, reproductive performance
and diet composition of marine birds in the southern California Current system, 1969–1997. Prog Oceanogr 49:
309–329
Tveraa T, Christensen GN (2002) Body condition and parental
decisions in the snow petrel (Pagodroma nivea). Auk 119:
266–270
Walker BG, Boersma PD (2003) Diving behavior of Magellanic penguins (Spheniscus magellanicus) at Punta
Tombo, Argentina. Can J Zool 81:1471–1483
Weimerskirch H, Salamolard M, Sarrazin F, Jouventin P
(1993) Foraging strategy of wandering albatrosses
through the breeding season: a study using satellite telemetry. Auk 110:325–342
Weimerskirch H, Chastel O, Ackermann L, Chaurand T,
Cuenot-Chaillet F, Hindermeyer X, Judas J (1994) Alternate long and short foraging trips in pelagic seabird
parents. Anim Behav 47:472–476
Williams TD (1995) The penguins. Oxford University Press,
Oxford
Wilson RP, Wilson MPTJ (1989) Tape: a package-attachment
technique for penguins. Wildl Soc Bull 17:77–79
Wilson RP, Scolaro JA, Grémillet D, Kierspel MAM and others
(2005) How do Magellanic penguins cope with variability
in their access to prey? Ecol Monogr 75:379–401
Yorio P, Boersma PD (1994) Causes of nest desertion during
incubation in the Magellanic penguin (Spheniscus magellanicus). Condor 96:1076–1083
Submitted: March 6, 2008; Accepted: September 19, 2008
Proofs received from author(s): December 24, 2008
Descargar