AJSLP Research Article Narratives About Autism: An Analysis of YouTube Videos by Individuals Who Self-Identify as Autistic Henry Angulo-Jiméneza,b and Laura DeThornea,c Purpose: The goal of this study was to expand the field’s understanding of autism through the analysis of 1st-person perspectives from autistic video webloggers (vloggers). Method: This study analyzed the representation of autism in 39 YouTube videos authored by self-identified autistic individuals and published between 2007 and 2015. Consistent with the cross-disciplinary tradition of narrative inquiry, thematic analyses of the video transcripts were conducted. Findings: Vloggers were predominantly, but not exclusively, White male adults who spoke mainstream American English and self-identified as experiencing Asperger’s syndrome. Key findings included (a) the predominance of a narrative about autism that incorporated features of both the medical model of disability and the neurodiversity paradigm to varying degrees, (b) a trend toward more medical model features across most content areas, and (c) a relatively high prevalence of neurodiversity paradigm features related specifically to language use and the description of autistic traits. Conclusions: Implications include the need for clinicians to (a) familiarize themselves with the varying views of autism held within the autistic community, (b) reflect on the language used to talk about autism and listen to how clients and/or their caregivers talk about it, and (c) consider the exploration of potential positive dimensions of autistic traits. B autistic individuals currently on an SLP’s caseload, many of whom are still children, recent years have brought a blossoming of autistic voices and communities on the Internet through weblogs, listservers, forums, and so forth (Brownlow & O’Dell, 2006; Davidson, 2008; Donaldson et al., 2017; Pellicano & Stears, 2011). This study aims to capitalize on this rich Internet presence to enhance the field’s understanding of autism by extracting first-person perspectives from online videos where individuals who self-identify as autistic share their experience of autism with YouTube viewers. Specifically, the study analyzed 39 video accounts of life on the spectrum to characterize the vloggers and establish their representation of autism. The study thus answered the following questions: Who are the vloggers? And do the videos ased on the rise in autism prevalence rates (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2017), speechlanguage pathologists (SLPs) are increasingly serving autistic individuals,1 their families, caregivers, and other related stakeholders across a variety of settings (Donaldson, Krejcha, & McMillin, 2017). Even though evidence-based practice requires the integration of client/stakeholder “needs, abilities, values, preferences, and interests” (American SpeechLanguage-Hearing Association [ASHA], 2004, 2005), firstperson perspectives regarding life on the spectrum are relatively limited in the scientific literature (DePape & Lindsay, 2016). Direct knowledge from autistic individuals is central to providing quality clinical services (ASHA, 2005) and for helping interpret academic findings (Pellicano & Stears, 2011). In addition to the valuable perspectives of a Department of Speech and Hearing Science, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign b Escuela de Lenguas Modernas, Universidad de Costa Rica at San José c Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, Western Michigan University at Kalamazoo Correspondence to Henry Angulo-Jiménez: [email protected] Editor-in-Chief: Julie Barkmeier-Kraemer Editor: Erinn Finke Received March 6, 2018 Revision received June 26, 2018 Accepted October 19, 2018 https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-18-0045 1 Although the authors recognize and appreciate differing opinions regarding the use of person-first versus identity-first language, the latter has been used in this article because this study was informed by the neurodiversity paradigm, whose advocates prefer identity-first language (Bagatell, 2010; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Kapp, GillespieLynch, Sherman, & Hutman, 2013; Oakley, 2012; Peers, SpencerCavaliere, & Eales, 2014; Pollak, 2009; Sinclair, 1999, 2013). This position is consistent with the American Psychological Association recommendation that authors respect “what people prefer to be called” (American Psychological Association, 2010, p. 72). Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time of publication. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 • Copyright © 2019 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 569 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss represent autism as a disorder (medical model), a difference (neurodiversity paradigm), or both? This study was motivated by the recognition of varying representations of autism within the autistic/autism community.2 Despite the existence of a plethora of approaches to disability, this study focused on the two representations of autism that have been predominant in the autism literature (Donaldson et al., 2017; Gillespie-Lynch, Kapp, Brooks, Pickens, & Schwartzman, 2017; Kapp, GillespieLynch, Sherman, & Hutman, 2013). Specifically, the study concentrated on the medical model of disability, which depicts autism as a biologically given neurological disorder, and the contrasting neurodiversity paradigm, which portrays autism as a neurological difference. Accordingly, a brief overview of the canonical forms of the medical model of disability and the neurodiversity paradigm is provided below. The Medical Model of Disability The medical model of disability dates back to the 18th and early 19th centuries and is prominent among nonautistic laypeople, researchers, professionals from many fields, and some autistic individuals (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013; Areheart, 2008; Bricher, 2000; Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2017; Hayasaki, 2015; Shyman, 2016). In this model, disability is conceptualized as the pathological absence, deficit, excess, or delay in function and/or form, with respect to some normative standards (APA, 2013; Davis, 2013; Shakespeare, 2013; Sisti, 2015; Strauss, 2013). Such deviation from the norm is thought to be centered within the person’s body and/or mind (Areheart, 2008; Shyman, 2016; Strauss, 2013). Disability is thereby a biological intrinsic condition that is to be prevented, detected early in life, and cured or at least ameliorated (Bagatell, 2010; Haegele & Hodge, 2016; Shyman, 2016; Strauss, 2013). Health professionals, particularly doctors and mainstream scientists, are considered “cognitive authorities” in disability matters, and they are assumed to provide the necessary expertise related to cause, diagnosis, and intervention (Brisenden, 1986; Giangreco, 2004; Haegele & Hodge, 2016, p. 197). Accordingly, autistic individuals are expected to seek a process of normalization, reduction of symptoms, and/or “adaptation and learning to function despite their disability” (Donaldson et al., 2017; Goering, 2015, p. 134; Kapp et al., 2013; Strauss, 2013). Not deferring to professional judgment is often regarded as psychological denial, refusal to comply, or lack of motivation (Haegele & Hodge, 2016). 2 In this document, a distinction between the autistic and autism communities is established. The former refers to the group of individuals on the spectrum, whereas the latter, as defined in Donaldson et al. (2017), includes not only autistic people but also “their parents and/or caregivers, friends and family members, professionals serving these individuals, and other allies and advocates” (p. 56), as well as autism researchers. 570 The medical model view of autism as a pathology is apparent in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, which is “the standard classification of mental disorders used in clinical and community settings” (Paul, 2013, para. 3) and guides diagnostic decisions by clinicians worldwide, including SLPs (ASHA, 2010, 2011, 2012a, 2012b, n.d.). The APA classifies autism—or autism spectrum disorders,3 to use its terminology—as a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by “persistent deficits (emphasis added) in social communication and social interaction across multiple contexts, including deficits in social reciprocity, nonverbal communicative behaviors used for social interaction, and skills in developing, maintaining, and understanding relationships” (APA, 2013, “Neurodevelopmental Disorders”). These limitations in social communication and interaction are paired with excesses that manifest themselves as “restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities” (APA, 2013, “Neurodevelopmental Disorders”). The influence of the medical model—more specifically of the research done and funded within this theoretical framework—has resulted in greater awareness and understanding of autism and the challenges that come with it (Pellicano, Dinsmore, & Charman, 2014; Pellicano & Stears, 2011). Moreover, the medical model has provided a diagnostic label and a detailed clinical profile that allows for access to specific accommodations and services, not to mention the validation and increased self-understanding that autistic individuals themselves report to attain when they obtain an autism diagnosis (Huws & Jones, 2008). Despite these perceived benefits, the canonical form of the medical model has been charged with a disregard for the sociopsychological aspects of disability, a focus on the negative dimension of disability, an overfocus on diagnosis, and an imbalance regarding the influence that medical personnel and disabled individuals have on courses of treatment (Brisenden, 1986; Haegele & Hodge, 2016). These and other critiques may at least in part explain the emergence of other models such as the social model of disability and its several reformulations, as well as other disability interpretation frameworks theoretically related with it, including the neurodiversity paradigm. The Neurodiversity Paradigm The neurodiversity paradigm started to develop in the late 1990s, and it is now favored by many autistic people and some scholars and professionals (Donaldson et al., 2017; Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2017). This approach to the understanding of autism is sometimes considered an outgrowth of the social model of disability, given the number of shared ideas (Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2017; Shakespeare, 2013). Despite their evident conceptual overlap, the connection between the social model and the neurodiversity 3 This diagnostic category merges the former autistic disorder, Asperger’s disorder, and pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified (APA, 2013). American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss paradigm is rarely made explicit in recent texts about neurodiversity, which most frequently tie the latter to the autism rights movement (Armstrong, 2010; Grant, 2009; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Kapp et al., 2013; Silberman, 2015; Strauss, 2013). In his 2015 book about the history of autism, Silberman claims that the neurodiversity paradigm started to develop when a group of autistic individuals realized that many of the challenges they faced could be addressed through environmental accommodations and were not inherent to their neurological differences. The Internet,4 free from the constraints and demands of interaction in more traditional social environments, proved an ideal space for autistic individuals to reach out to each other and share their ideas both with other autistic people and with groups experiencing other forms of neurodivergence (Bagatell, 2010; Davidson, 2008; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Kapp et al., 2013; Sinclair, 2010). Today, the neurodiversity paradigm and its related civil rights movement bring together and represent not only people on the spectrum but also individuals from other neurominorities such as those who experience dyslexia, attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder, and stuttering (Armstrong, 2010; Brownlow, 2010; Grant, 2009; Walker, 2014a; Watermeyer & Kathard, 2016). A central tenet of the neurodiversity paradigm is the perception of autistic brains as manifestations of the ordinary variation found across the human species (Jaarsma & Welin, 2012). For this reason, considering autistic brains abnormal is akin to considering certain skin colors and sexual orientations anomalous. In other words, autistic brains are perceived as different, not disordered. Furthermore, the challenges individuals on the spectrum face are thought to stem primarily from society’s privileging of nonautistic over autistic traits (behaviors, abilities, communication, etc.) and the subsequent design of environments that do not support autistic brains/bodies (Armstrong, 2010; Autistic SelfAdvocacy Network [ASAN], 2016; Dewsbury, Clarke, Dave, Rouncefield, & Sommerville, 2004; Goodley, 2001; Robertson, 2011; Strauss, 2013). The representation of autism as a difference rather than a disorder has at least three implications. First is resistance to focusing efforts on attempts to cure autism (Armstrong, 2010; Grant, 2009; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Parasole, 2015; Pellicano & Stears, 2011; Silvers, 2009; Walker, 2013, 2014a, 2014b). Second is the call for recontextualization, redefinition, and revalorization of autism indicators (Bagatell, 2010). For instance, the “excessive preoccupation with local coherence” regarded as a symptom of autism in the medical model is often reinterpreted within the framework of the neurodiversity paradigm as “an unusual and distinctive ability to attend to details on their own terms, not subsumed into a larger totality” (Strauss, 2013, p. 467). 4 The Internet for the autistic community has been homologized to sign language for the deaf community (Brownlow & O’Dell, 2006; Davidson, 2008). The last implication of representing autism as a natural and valued form of diversity is its potential for constructing identity. Advocates of the neurodiversity paradigm encourage autistic individuals to claim autism (Strauss, 2013) as an integral part of their identity and to celebrate it (Bagatell, 2010; Kapp et al., 2013). This development of positive identity, in tandem with the possibilities offered by online spaces, has led to the emergence of an autistic culture/community analogous to Black, Queer, and Deaf culture/community (Bagatell, 2010; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Strauss, 2013). Autistic culture has and is developed through different “tools of identity” and “communitybuilding mechanisms” (Bagatell, 2010, p. 39) such as (a) rituals and events (e.g., Autistic Pride Day, the Autreat), (b) organizations and enterprises run by autistic people (e.g., Autism Network International, ASAN, Autonomous Press), (c) artifacts (t-shirts, books, stimming toys, slogans), (d) shared vocabulary (e.g., neuroqueer, Aspie, autistic cousin), and (e) distinctive autistic styles of communication (Davidson, 2008; Donaldson et al., 2017). Like other social rights movements, the neurodiversity paradigm has met opposition from both autistic and nonautistic people, including parents, clinicians, and scientists (Bagatell, 2010; Parasole, 2015). It has been argued that the neurodiversity paradigm privileges the experiences and views of individuals on the spectrum who are less affected by autism (Bagatell, 2010; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Parasole, 2015). Also, some have argued that the neurodiversity paradigm, particularly its resistance to a cure, introduces its own form of oppression and may be just as harmful for autistic individuals as the medical model of disability (Parasole, 2015). Interestingly, though, Bagatell (2010) asserts that an autistic person’s stand regarding the cure issue does not seem to correlate with his or her level of support needs. A related controversy is the issue of who is “autistic enough” to represent other autistic people. Parents and professionals have argued that some autistic advocates are not affected enough by autism to represent the autistic community, yet by that same logic, nonautistic parents and professionals are not well positioned to represent the best interest of autistic individuals either (Endow, n.d.; Monje, 2015). Similar Studies Many qualitative studies conducted within a narrative research framework have focused on the perspective of nonautistic parents, social workers, and other stakeholders (DePape & Lindsay, 2016). The few qualitative studies that have centered on first-person perspectives from autistic individuals5 have investigated other specified topics including autistic sensory–perceptual experiences (e.g., Jones, 5 The search for studies similar to the one presented here focused on academic scholarship; thus, autobiographical books written by autistic individuals are not included in this discussion of the extant literature. See Chamak, Bonniau, Jaunay, and Cohen (2008) for an analysis of 19 of these books. Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism 571 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss Quigney, & Huws, 2003), perceptions of physical education classes (e.g., Healy, Msetfi, & Gallagher, 2013), effects of diagnosis and diagnosis disclosure (Huws & Jones, 2008), employment (Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2004), negotiation of university spaces (Madriaga, 2010), perceptions of friendship (Howard, Cohn, & Orsmond, 2006), and social challenges and supports (Muller, Schuler, & Yates, 2008). The interested reader is referred to DePape and Lindsay (2016) for a qualitative metasynthesis of these studies. Five studies that, like this study, had a wider scope, investigating the lived experience of autism more holistically, were located. These studies were published in journals and by authors in special education, psychology, and rehabilitation studies. The researchers collected data in both physical and virtual spaces from autistic individuals, with ages ranging from 10 to 35 years. Reasons for collecting data online included the potential advantage of the Internet over other environments when it comes to working with hard-to-reach groups, the reported greater ease with which many autistic individuals communicate online as opposed to face-to-face, and the central role of the Internet in enabling autistic users to share experiences and perceptions (Brownlow & O’Dell, 2006; Jones, Zahl, & Huws, 2001). Data took the form of answers to interview questions, field notes, postings in online discussion groups, and artifacts. These data were submitted to thematic and/or discursive analyses whose results were in one of the cases shared with an external autistic consultant (Brownlow, 2010; Brownlow & O’Dell, 2006; Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2002; R. S. P. Jones, Huws, & Beck, 2013; McLaughlin & Rafferty, 2014). Key findings from the five studies included autistic people as cognitive authorities, differences between autistic and nonautistic individuals as well as among autistic participants, and mixed feelings about autism. In three of the studies, the researchers found that autistic individuals perceive themselves as the real experts on autism, arguing that only someone on the spectrum “can truly know and understand what autism is” (Brownlow & O’Dell, 2006; Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2002; R. S. P. Jones et al., 2013, p. 138). Comparisons between autistic and nonautistic people highlighted differences in neurotype, communication styles, predictability, flexibility, and talent of the individuals in both groups (Brownlow, 2010; Brownlow & O’Dell, 2006; Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2002). Comparisons among autistic individuals both acknowledged the uniqueness of every person’s experience of autism and highlighted shared traits (R. S. P. Jones et al., 2013). Finally, a perception of autism as an entity that both gives and takes away and the mixed feelings associated with such perception were most apparent in the study of McLaughlin and Rafferty (2014). In this study, the participating teenagers claimed, for instance, that autism had suppressed their ability to connect with others, but at the same time, they claimed their condition had provided them with something to make up for their losses, for example, interest in certain topics and gifts for certain school subjects (McLaughlin & Rafferty, 2014). 572 This study has similarities and differences with prior research. First, like previous studies, it purports to add to the increasing (Davidson, 2008; Humphrey & Lewis, 2008), albeit still relatively scarce, scientific literature focusing exclusively on first-person perspectives of autism. Second, following the steps of other researchers, it included an external autistic consultant to help avoid “a diagnostic overshadowing interpretation” of data and counterproductive neurotypical bias (Huws & Jones, 2008; R. S. P. Jones et al., 2013, p. 134; Kirby, Dickie, & Baranek, 2014; Preece & Jordan, 2009). Finally, considering the positive engagement of autistic individuals with the Internet and other researchers’ call for more autism research in virtual spaces, in this study, the investigators have opted for collecting data from the Internet, instead of gathering information in a more traditional, face-to-face environment (Brownlow, 2010; Davidson, 2008; Jones et al., 2003). Regarding its unique contributions, this study is, to the best of our knowledge, the first study within communication sciences and disorders (CSD) that used narrative inquiry to examine first-person perspectives on autism. Also, the videos analyzed in this study were neither elicited by the researchers nor produced in the (virtual) presence of a (neurotypical) investigator; they are thus expected to be less filtered by a researcher’s perspective (i.e., more authentic in qualitative terms). Finally, whereas other studies have collected data from discussion posts (Brownlow & O’Dell, 2006) and homepage websites (Jones et al., 2003), this is the first study to analyze video weblog entries. A vlog is a weblog (or blog) that contains video content (Sharpened Productions, n.d.). The term vlog can refer to a whole online collection of videos or to a single video in such collection. In this document, the word vlog will be used to denote the whole assortment of video weblogs, and the words video and vlog entry will be used to refer to a specific video in a vlog. In vlogs, individuals, most frequently from younger generations, share their opinions and feelings about different topics, almost always in a natural “diary-like form” (Holtgrave, 2015, p. 177; Sanchez-Cortes, Kumano, Otsuka, & Gatica-Perez, 2015; Teijeiro-Mosquera, Biel, Alba-Castro, & Gatica-Perez, 2015). Because most vlog entries feature at least the torso and face of the vlogger, researchers can have access to nonverbal aspects of communication, which can in turn assist in data transcription and analysis. This is an advantage of vlog data over text data coming from personal webpages and discussion lists. Method Narrative Research Given the researchers’ interest in establishing how the vloggers represented autism in their videos, a qualitative research design was deemed most appropriate. Furthermore, because the vlog entries were nonliterary, autobiographical accounts of the experience of autism over time (i.e., nonfictional narratives), a narrative inquiry approach was selected. This decision was also consistent with the practice in American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss other health disciplines such as medicine, nursing, and psychology (Holloway & Freshwater, 2007; Hurwitz, Greenhalgh, & Skultans, 2004; Levitt et al., 2018; Polkinghorne, 1988, 2005; Squire, Andrews, & Tamboukou, 2013). The meaning of narrative here is threefold. It is conceived of as a mental process that allows human beings to make sense of events and experiences, often taking note of explanations that are readily available in society (grand narratives) and accepting, resisting, and/or manipulating them (Holloway & Freshwater, 2007; Polkinghorne, 2005; Webster & Mertova, 2007). A narrative is also understood as the concrete product of that mental process: an experiential account and a window into the meaning given to experience (Cortazzi, 1993; Polkinghorne, 2005). Finally, narrative is taken as synonymous with discourse, a stance toward a given phenomenon that is accessible through linguistic expression (Polkinghorne, 1988). These three understandings of narrative are embraced in this study, and it is thereby assumed that the videos in the data are the tangible expressions of a process through which the vloggers have come to forge their understanding of autism. Research Team The research team consisted of the two authors, an external autistic consultant, and eight research assistants. Both authors self-identify as neurotypical, and one of them has an autistic family member. The first author identifies himself as a Costa Rican man with a background in teaching English as a foreign language and linguistics and as a doctoral student of speech and hearing science. The second author is a White professor in CSD and an SLP who selfidentifies as a European American woman. The external consultant identifies himself as a White autistic man with an academic background in human–computer interaction and information sciences and technology. He is an independent researcher with extensive experience advancing disability policy, practice, and research. He has served as a disability consultant at both state and federal levels, and he is also one of the founders of the ASAN. The eight research assistants all self-identify as neurotypical and female, and they were all pursuing undergraduate degrees in CSD at the time of the study. Data Collection Multiple online searches were conducted by different members of the research team to locate publicly available vlog entries whose title was suggestive of the presentation of an account of autism and/or life with autism. Initial search key words included autism, Asperger’s, PDD, ASD, life, what, like, my, story, experience, and autistic. These words were combined into phrases such as My story of autism, What it is like to have autism, My life with Asperger’s, Being autistic, and so forth. Other key words and phrases such as aspie and living with autism were added as the search progressed. The multiple searches retrieved a total of 46 candidate videos, which were then watched in full by the first author and at least one other member of the research team to determine if they met the inclusion criteria. That is, the candidate vlog entries were inspected to establish if they were (a) an experiential account of autism and/or life with it, (b) authored by individuals who self-identified as autistic, (c) featured in the vlogger’s own YouTube channel,6 (d) recorded in English, and (e) uploaded between 2005, when YouTube was launched, and 2015 (Alleyne, 2008), when this study began. Compliance of the candidate video with the inclusion criteria was further assessed (a) by visiting the Home and About sections of the vlog channel where the candidate video was featured and (b) during the transcription consensus pass. Six videos were excluded because they were featured in the channel of another person or organization and not in the channel of the self-identified autistic vlogger. When two vlog entries on the topic of interest were featured in the same vlog, the more recent video was kept. For this reason, one of the 46 candidate videos was replaced by another vlog entry by the same vlogger. This process resulted in a total of N = 39 videos from 39 different vlogs in the study’s database. Of the 39 videos that met the established inclusion criteria, 95% (37/39) were uploaded between 2010 and 2015. The oldest video was uploaded in 2007. The vlog entries ranged in length from 2 min 15 s to 39 min 13 s, with a mean duration of approximately 12 min. The corresponding transcripts ranged in length from 15 to 352 lines of transcription, with a mean of 105 lines and a standard deviation of 66 lines. Consistent with the practice of narrative inquiry (Polkinghorne, 1988), there was no attempt to control the length or specified content of the videos. Descriptive information regarding the videos can be found in Table 1. Data Transcription Given the importance of the trustworthiness of transcripts for dependability of the research findings in narrative inquiry (Webster & Mertova, 2007), particular attention was paid to the transcription of the vlog entries. Each video was transcribed verbatim and in full—including identifying information—through a two-pass process. Transcription conventions and guidelines were developed by the research team and/or drawn from established resources (e.g., Hengst, 2003; Miller & Long, 2005; SimmonsMackie & Damico, 1999), and they were piloted on available vlog entries over a 6-month period to maximize ease of use, agreement across transcribers, and representation of important linguistic and paralinguistic features of the videos. Table 2, which transcribers used for quick reference, presents the abbreviated version of the transcription conventions and guidelines implemented in the study. The first transcription pass was completed by one research assistant in charge of transcribing the video, whereas the second (consensus) pass was completed between this research 6 YouTube was chosen given its reported popularity among autistic individuals (Davidson, 2008). Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism 573 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss Table 1. Information about the videos and their corresponding transcripts. Vlogger Adrian Angel Anna Armando Arwen Becky Cher Dolly Emilia Eva Hosh Jake Jason Jerry John Jonathan Julio Ken Luis Luis Ángel Matt Marcus Mike Morton Nelson Nicole Oscar Porter Ran Roberto Rocco Ruth Ryan Steve Teresa Toby Travis Willy Y. C. Year video was uploaded Length of the video (min:s) Lines of transcription 2012 2015 2014 2009 2011 2014 2015 2010 2012 2012 2015 2013 2014 2010 2015 2014 2013 2012 2014 2015 2010 2014 2011 2010 2015 2012 2014 2011 2013 2011 2012 2013 2012 2007 2013 2013 2013 2014 2012 06:08 12:38 15:37 07:30 08:40 12:05 18:04 09:40 14:37 02:34 03:10 06:46 03:12 05:37 21:14 15:31 10:01 03:56 07:22 02:48 18:04 17:17 08:57 21:57 15:41 08:18 11:44 02:15 05:16 06:43 13:59 26:31 04:21 22:53 39:13 07:12 10:53 18:04 18:12 42 135 117 82 69 112 181 122 122 25 15 36 31 59 195 115 112 42 70 28 149 153 92 188 181 65 98 18 69 69 166 199 45 101 352 78 107 134 124 assistant and the first author. Transcription disagreements that could not be resolved in the consensus pass were discussed during weekly research team meetings. During this time, the whole research team would watch the problematic stretch of video and agree on its transcription. When agreement was not possible, the corresponding utterance was judged unintelligible and denoted as such in the video transcript. Data Analysis As is common practice in qualitative research, the resulting transcripts were submitted to content-based thematic analyses (Aronson, 1995; Braun & Clarke, 2006; Boyatzis, 1998; Fereday & Muir-Cochraine, 2006; Guest, MacQueen, & Namey, 2012) to characterize the vloggers and the vlog entries according to their alignment with the medical model or the neurodiversity paradigm narrative about autism. In this study, all themes and their corresponding codes were established a priori based on (a) the researchers’ interest in particular demographic information 574 and (b) the descriptions of the medical model of disability and the neurodiversity paradigm predominant in the autism literature. The themes and their codes were therefore researcher/theory driven and applied deductively (Braun & Clarke, 2006; Boyatzis, 1998; Polkinghorne, 1995). In the following two sections, more information about when and how the themes were established and applied is presented. Research Question 1: Vlogger Demographic Information Generally comparable to the concept of external validity in quantitative research, the qualitative concept of transferability refers to the extent to which the findings of a qualitative study can be transferred to other contexts or settings (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Providing enough information to enable readers “contemplating application in another setting to make the needed comparisons of similarity” is the unique responsibility of the researcher (Levitt et al., 2018; Webster & Mertova, 2007, p. 101;). The vloggers were demographically characterized to provide the audience American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss Table 2. Transcription conventions abridged. Aspect Abandoned word Abandoned utterance Duration of video End of 1-min stretch of video Font Identification information Lengthening Loudness Line number Margins Nonverbal actions contributing to meaning Page numbers Pauses (organic) Pauses (filled) Pauses (two seconds or less) Pauses (more than two seconds) Repetition of word(s) Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism Repetitive automatic, uncontrolled, or unconscious behaviors Running head contents Slurring Spacing Sustained action Syllable-by-syllable enunciation Title of script Transcriber comments Transcriber information Unintelligible words Whispering Guideline/mark/info to include Use * to mark an abandoned WORD Use > to mark an abandoned utterance (false starts) Write it on the line following the last line of transcription. Flush right, boldface, and make it size 18. Use boldfaced, size 18 /// to mark the end of every 1-min stretch of transcription Use Times New Roman 12 Preceding the transcript body, record the following information: name, online identity, age, gender, race/ethnicity, dialect, self-identification, reported diagnosis, and video URL. Write UNSPECIFIED when the vlogger does not explicitly mention name, online identity, age, self-identification, or reported diagnosis in any of the agreed-upon sources. If gender, dialect, and race/ethnicity are not specified by the vlogger, record your own perception. Indicate this information is perceived by writing (perceived) in parenthesis following the corresponding piece of info. Use : to mark lengthening CAPITALIZE syllable, word, phrase, etc. that is louder Add to transcript body ONLY Use 1-inch margins Enclose in square brackets all those actions that contribute to meaning attained through verbal action Insert at the right bottom side of the page Use regular orthographic marks (commas, periods, semicolons, dashes, etc.) Spell them out and set them off between commas Use ellipsis Use ellipsis + Greek number (length of pause) + ellipsis Enclose repeated word(s) in parentheses so that all the parts of the utterance remain together and the repetitions are set off in parentheses on the left. Use footnotes to call attention to repetitive behaviors perceived as divorced from verbal utterances, i.e., not evidently produced to add meaning to what is said. Title of video/name of channel/country/upload date Use condensed spacing between letters Double space the document Underline stretch of speech through which action is sustained CAPITALIZE and mark separations with hyphens Center and boldface the title of the video. Report title as included in the vlog entry. Use footnotes for low-level inferences, hunches, interpretations, observations, connections with literature, etc. Following duration line. Justify and boldface. Include name and last date transcription was revised. Use Xs to indicate unintelligible words. Italicize Example Many au* Aspies agree with that. Many Aspies> I mean, we all agree. That’s all. I guess. 00:03:11 N/A N/A N/A So:, I said “no:”. N/A N/A N/A I am “normal” [rolls his eyes]. N/A N/A I, um, didn’t mean to say that. I didn’t…mean to say that. I didn’t…3…mean to say that. (I didn’t) I didn’t mean to say that. I love videogames, series…7 N/A Condensed spacing looks like this. N/A I am “normal” [giggles] PRO-CRAS-TI-NA-TING A taste of Asperger/autism First I will tell you about my childhood.8 00:03:11 Transcriber: Henry Angulo Last Revision: 10/30/2016 I’m a XXX fan. I watch Ghost Whisperer. Note. N/A = Not Apply. 575 7 Licks his lips. Other examples include swaying, playing with hair, rocking, etc. 8 Seems to be reading from an outline as he constantly looks down. Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions of this document with such information and to address, at least in part, the aforementioned contentious discussions regarding whose voices are in the best position to represent the autistic community (Monje, 2015). The researchers were particularly interested in the vloggers’ age at the time the vlog entry was posted, gender, country, dialect, race/ethnicity, self-identification (e.g., autistic, Aspie), and reported diagnosis—all of which can be aspects of identity that shape lived experience. Transcripts were coded deductively and manually for age, gender, country, dialect, race/ethnicity, self-identification, and reported diagnosis by the first author and the research assistant who created the original transcription. When any of the information of interest was not volunteered by the vloggers in the videos themselves, it was obtained from the vlog entry’s description or from the “About” section of the vlog. The information provided in the video itself took precedent vis-à-vis contradictory information volunteered in the other two sources. Explicit statements about gender, race/ethnicity, and dialect were usually absent in these three sources. The first author and the research assistants therefore relied on perceptions of gender, race/ethnicity, and dialect to characterize the vloggers and noted these perceptions in the transcripts (see transcription conventions and guidelines in Table 2). Disagreements as to any of these perceived categories during the consensus pass were discussed in weekly meetings. During this time, the whole research team would watch the video together and come to an agreement on the vlogger’s (perceived) gender, race/ethnicity, and dialect. The first author entered the transcripts and associated codes into Atlas.ti, a software package used in the analysis of qualitative data (Scientific Software Development GmbH, n.d.). Research Question 2: Vlogger Representation of Autism To address the question of autism representation, video transcripts were analyzed to identify features of the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm in them. The analysis was based on predominant descriptions of both approaches to autism available in the theoretical and empirical literature (e.g., Armstrong, 2010; Bagatell, 2010; Donaldson et al., 2017; Haegele & Hodge, 2016; Jaarsma & Welin, 2012; Kapp et al., 2013; Pollak, 2009; Sisti, 2015; Strauss, 2013; Walker, 2014a). Specifically, the first author identified a set of 12 content areas upon which the medical model of disability and the neurodiversity paradigm can be said to differ. Through a process of triangulated discussion (Levitt et al., 2018) among the authors and the external autistic consultant, these 12 content areas were reduced to nine that helped lessen redundancy and best captured distinctions between the medical model and the neurodiversity approach to autism: (a) language, (b) identified source of the challenges faced by autistic individuals, (c) description of autistic traits, (d) position toward the concept of normality, (e) established types of people, (f) conception of autism in relation to the person, (g) preferred course of action upon autism and autistic traits, (h) explicit definition of autism, and (i) allocation of autism expertise. Each of the nine 576 content areas was operationalized into two contrasting features that were thought to distinguish the medical model from the neurodiversity paradigm, thereby yielding a total of 18 contrasting—albeit not mutually exclusive—features (nine associated with the medical model and nine associated with the neurodiversity paradigm). The established contrasting features, which are presented in Table 3, were subsequently reviewed and modified with the second author and the external consultant as well. At this point, it is worth reiterating that the contrasting features within each content area and across models were not necessarily mutually exclusive. To illustrate, vloggers could use—and in fact used—language associated with both a disorder (medical model) and a difference (neurodiversity paradigm) when referencing autism within the same video. Both contrasting features were therefore recognized and documented in the vlog entries accordingly. It is also worth acknowledging that some of the content areas and their associated contrasting features were intricately related. Despite this, they were distinct enough as to be considered separately as done in this study and in the literature upon which they were established. The 18 contrasting features associated with the nine content areas worked as preestablished, literature-driven descriptive themes and were laid out in the instrument presented in Table 4, which also contained examples and general instructions to guide the analyses. The first author used this instrument to establish the presence or absence of the 18 contrasting features in the vlog entries. The comments column was used to note nonoverlapping supporting evidence (e.g., direct quotations) for the presence of a given contrasting feature in the vlog entries. These and other pieces of nonoverlapping supporting evidence were also identified by the first author in the digital transcripts stored in Atlas.ti, and they were assigned a related code for easy retrieval, for example, language, source of challenges, types of people, definition of autism, and so forth. Importantly, stretches of text, be it words or multisentential extracts, that were used as evidence of the presence of a given contrasting feature were not used to support the occurrence of another contrasting feature. Debriefing sessions (Levitt et al., 2018; Lincoln & Guba, 1985) between the first and second authors were conducted weekly to discuss the rationale for associating stretches of transcript with a given contrasting feature, thus enhancing the groundedness of the research findings in the data. Findings Characterization of the Vloggers The 39 vloggers included 26 perceived men and 13 perceived women. More than half of the vloggers (n = 21) did not specify how old they were when the video was recorded; however, the 18 vloggers who did ranged in age from 13 to 45 years (M = 24 years, SD = 8). To establish the vloggers’ possible origin within a geographical area and racial/ethnic group, the researchers drew on perceived dialect and perceived race/ethnicity as well as on information American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss Table 3. Contrasting features of the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm organized by content area. Content area Language Source of challenges Description of autistic traits Position toward the concept of normality Types of people Conception of autism in relation to the person Preferred course of action Explicit definition of autism Allocation of autism expertise Medical model Neurodiversity paradigm Uses language (that can be) associated with a disorder narrative about autism Claims that the challenges faced by autistic individuals stem from autism Describes autistic traits with words that suggest deficit, delay, defect, and/or excess Assumes normality exists and presents it as desirable Separates individuals into “normal people” and “people with autism” Conceives of autism as added on top of/ separable from the “real person” Emphasizes/favors prevention, detection, cure, and/or amelioration of autism/autistic traits Defines/refers to autism as a disorder and to autistic brains as abnormal, deviant, defective, and/or disordered Presents nonautistic health professionals, scientists, and other neurotypical individuals as the experts on autism Uses language (that can be) associated with a difference narrative about autism Claims that the challenges faced by autistic people stem from societal privileging of nonautistic brains/traits Contextualizes, redefines, and/or revalorizes autistic traits Contextualizes, problematizes, and/or resists normality Separates individuals into autistic and nonautistic/ neurotypical people Conceives of autism as integral to/inseparable from the autistic person Emphasizes/Favors accommodation, inclusion, and acceptance of autistic individuals Defines/refers to autism as a neurological difference and to autistic brains as different, distinct Presents autistic people as the experts on autism Table 4. Instrument to identify features of the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm in the vlogs. Featurea Yes (✓)/No (X) Comments Medical model 1 Words and lexical constructions that are associated by scholars with a disorder narrative about autism, or that can be associated with it given their connotation, are used (e.g., disorder, deficit). 2 The challenges faced by people on the spectrum are assumed to stem primarily from the intrinsic features of autism.b 3 Autistic traits are described with words that suggest deficit (e.g., poor), delay (e.g., be behind), defect (e.g., wrong), and/or excess (e.g., too + adjective). 4 Normality is assumed to exist and presented as desirable. 5 People are separated into normal people and people with autism.c 6 Autism is conceived of as an entity added on top of, and separable from, the real person. 7 Prevention, detection, cure, and/or amelioration of autism/autistic traits is emphasized/ favored over accommodation, inclusion, and acceptance of autism/autistic traits. 8 Autism is defined/referred to as a neurological disorder, and autistic brains are referred to as abnormal, deviant, defective, and/or disordered. 9 Nonautistic health professionals, scientists, and other neurotypical individuals are depicted as the experts on autism. Neurodiversity paradigm 1 Words and lexical constructions that are associated by scholars with a difference narrative about autism, or that can be associated with it given their connotation, are used (e.g., neurotypical, aspie). 2 The challenges faced by people on the spectrum are based on the privileging of nonautistic brains/traits.b 3 Autistic traits are contextualized (e.g., specifying when a trait is triggered and when not), redefined (e.g., as gifts), and/or revalorized (e.g., by presenting their positive sides). 4 The concept of normality is contextualized, problematized, and/or resisted. 5 People are separated into “autistic” and “neurotypical” or “nonautistic.”c 6 Autism is seen as integral to, thereby inseparable from, the autistic person. 7 Accommodation, inclusion, and acceptance of autistic individuals are emphasized/favored over detection, prevention, cure, and/or amelioration of autism/autistic traits. 8 Autism is explicitly defined/referred to as a neurological difference, and autistic brains are referred to as different, distinct. 9 Autistic individuals are depicted as the experts on autism. a Do not use the same piece of evidence for two or more contrasting features. bLook for the establishment of explicit causal links between experienced difficulties and autism/societal barriers. cOr any other categories that (do not) imply that one group represents the standard or rule. Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism 577 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss about the country the vloggers were targeting in their vlog (as specified in the “Location” descriptor of the “About” section of the video channel). Most of the vloggers (n = 23) were perceived as speaking mainstream American English; the remainder were perceived as speakers of British English (n = 5), Australian English (n = 2), Irish English (n = 1), African American English (n = 1), Spanish-influenced American English (n = 1), Southern American English (n = 1), Canadian English (n = 1), and English as a second language (n = 1; the vlogger reported Swedish to be his first language). The team could not reach consensus on the specific dialect of the three remaining vloggers, aside from European non–British English dialect. In terms of race/ ethnicity, most of the vloggers were perceived as White (n = 33); two, as Latino/Hispanic; two, as Asian American; and one, as African American. The race/ethnicity of one of the vloggers was not specified because she did not appear in the video; her video consisted of a sequence of drawings with narration. Finally, 16 vlogs had not been set to a specific country; of those that were, 13 were set to the United States, five were set to the United Kingdom/Great Britain, two were set to Canada, and one was set to each of the following: Australia, Denmark, and Ireland. Regarding self-identification9 and diagnosis, the vloggers reported experiencing Asperger’s syndrome (n = 20), autism (n = 11), high-functioning autism (n = 3), pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified (n = 3), mild autism (n = 1), and borderline Asperger’s (n = 1). Of the 24 vloggers who reported receiving a professional diagnosis, specified diagnoses included autism (n = 10), Asperger’s syndrome (n = 10), pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified (n = 3), and borderline Asperger’s (n = 1). One vlogger, Ruth, stated that she had never been diagnosed. Age of diagnosis was reported by 13 vloggers and ranged from 2 to 36 years (M = 12 years, SD = 10). Co-occurring forms of neurodivergence reported by the vloggers included obsessive compulsive disorder and synesthesia (n = 1), anxiety and depression (n = 2), schizotypal behavior disorder (n = 1), sensory-processing disorder (n = 1), attention-deficit disorder (n = 1), and dermatillomania (n = 1). Demographic information on each of the vloggers is summarized in Table 5. Representation of Autism Representation of autism was understood in this study as the specific arrangement of characteristics of the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm present in each vlog entry and in the set of videos in the data as a whole. Almost a quarter of the vlog entries in the data, 23% (9/39), presented features of the medical model of disability exclusively; that is, they had one or more features of the medical model and no feature of the neurodiversity paradigm. None of the videos had only features of the neurodiversity 9 Despite the overlap between these diagnostic categories, because self-identification was one of the aspects the researchers were interested in, the vloggers’ own words were honored. 578 paradigm. The remaining 77% (30/39) of the vlog entries had features of both the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm in different combinations and proportions and were thus considered hybrids. Interestingly, although examining shifting trends over time was outside the scope of this study, there did not seem to be a tendency toward more or fewer features of the medical model or the neurodiversity paradigm across the data set. Though one could, for instance, expect more recent videos to exhibit fewer features of the medical model and more features of the neurodiversity paradigm, the data did not lend obvious support to such an expectation. To illustrate, Nelson’s10 video from 2015 exhibited as many features of the medical model as Armando’s 2009 video (seven in total). Likewise, Arwen’s 2011 vlog entry displayed five features of the neurodiversity paradigm, and Luis Angel’s 2015 video exhibited only four features of the same approach. Table 6 specifies the features of the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm identified in each video as well as summary statistics regarding how many total vlog entries presented each feature. Table 7 summarizes the information to be presented below for the reader to refer to it as necessary. The percentage and number of videos exhibiting the contrasting features of the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm are displayed in the third and fifth columns correspondingly. The sixth column presents the percentage and number of videos exhibiting both contrasting features simultaneously. Finally, the seventh column presents the percentage and number of videos that did not exhibit any of the features listed in the corresponding row. The reader is also referred to Table 2 to see the transcription conventions employed in the transcript extracts that will be used as examples in this section. Language In overview, 95% (37/39) of the vloggers used language that can be associated with a disorder narrative about autism and with the medical model of disability, and 62% (24/39) of the vloggers used language that can be associated with a difference perspective about autism and with the neurodiversity paradigm. Of interest, 56% (22/39) of the vloggers used language that can be associated with both a disorder and a difference account of autism within the same video, thus being simultaneously consistent with the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm. The use of language associated with a disorder narrative about autism was the most frequently occurring feature of the medical model of disability. Likewise, the use of language associated with a difference 10 After much consideration, the research team decided to assign pseudonyms to the vloggers rather than use their public online identities for two key reasons. First, even though IRB approval was not required for the present study given the public availability of the data, it is common practice in research to anonymize data to the extent possible to protect privacy and minimize any negative consequences to participants. Second, the potential vulnerability of the vloggers in terms of age and/or disability led us toward this more protective stance. We in no way intend to minimize or limit the voices represented herein. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss Table 5. Demographic characterization of the vloggers. Assigned pseudonym Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism Perceived gender Reported age Adrian Angel Male Female 19 Unspecified MAE MAE Asian American White AS Autism Yes: AS Yes: Autism Unspecified Unspecified Anna Armando Arwen Becky Female Male Female Female 22 Unspecified Unspecified Unspecified MAE MAE MAE MAE White White White White Female Unspecified European, non-BrE White No No No Yes: borderline Asperger’s Yes: AS NA NA 16 Unspecified Cher AS Autism AS Borderline Asperger’s AS Unspecified Dolly Emilia Eva Hosh Jake Female Female Female Female Male Unspecified 20 Unspecified Unspecified 13 White White White Unidentifiable Hispanic/Latino Autism PDD-NOS AS HFA AS Yes: Yes: Yes: No Yes: Unspecified 3 Unspecified NA 10 Jason Jerry John Jonathan Julio Ken Luis Luis Ángel Matt Marcus Mike Morton Nelson Nicole Oscar Male Male Male Male Male Male Male Male Male Male Male Male Male Female Male Unspecified 20 Unspecified Unspecified 33 24 30 Unspecified 17 Unspecified 19 Unspecified Unspecified 16 36 MAE MAE MAE European, non-BrE Spanish-influenced English BrE MAE Irish English MAE MAE MAE SAE Australian English MAE MAE MAE ESL BrE MAE AAE White White White White White White White White White White White White White White African American Autism Autism AS AS AS PDD-NOS AS AS Autism PDD-NOS Autism AS AS AS AS No Yes: autism No No No Yes: PDD-NOS No Yes: AS No Yes: PDD-NOS Yes: autism Yes: AS No No Yes: AS Perceived dialect Perceived race/ethnicity Selfidentification Reports diagnosis? AS PDD-NOS AS autism Age of reported diagnosis NA 2 NA NA 23 4 NA Unspecified Unspecified 2 10 Unspecified NA NA 36 Reports co-occurring form of neurodivergence? No Yes: synesthesia and OCD Yes: dermatillomania No No No Yes: schizotypal behavioral disorder No No No No No No No No No No No No No Yes: ADD No No No No No Yes: sensory processing disorder (table continues) 579 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions 580 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 Table 5. (Continued). Assigned pseudonym Perceived gender Reported age Perceived dialect Perceived race/ethnicity Selfidentification Reports diagnosis? Porter Ran Roberto Rocco Ruth Ryan Steve Teresa Female Male Male Male Female Male Male Female Unspecified 26 Unspecified Unspecified 45 Unspecified 28 Unspecified BrE BrE BrE MAE MAE MAE European, non-BrE Australian English White White Hispanic/Latino White White White White White Autism AS HFA AS HFA AS Autism AS Yes: No No Yes: No No Yes: Yes: AS AS Unspecified NA NA 13 NA NA 26 Unspecified Toby Travis Willy Male Male Male 23 Unspecified 24 MAE MAE MAE Asian American White White Autism Mild autism AS Yes: autism Yes: autism Yes: autism 6 2 Unspecified Y.C. Male 24 Canadian English White Autism Yes: autism Unspecified autism AS Age of reported diagnosis Reports co-occurring form of neurodivergence? No No No No No No No Yes: anxiety and depression No No Yes: anxiety and depression No Note. MAE = mainstream American English; AS = Asperger’s syndrome; OCD = obsessive compulsive disorder; NA = not applicable; BrE = British English; PDD-NOS = pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified; HFA = high-functioning autism; SAE = Southern American English; ADD = attention-deficit disorder; ESL = English as a second language; AAE = African American English. Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Table 6. Features of the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm in each video. Model Feature Vlogger Adrian Angel Anna Armando Arwen Becky Cher Dolly Emilia Eva Hosh Jake Jason Jerry John Jonathan Julio Ken Luis Luis Angel Matt Marcus Mike Morton Nelson Nicole Oscar Porter Ran Roberto Rocco Ruth Ryan Steve Teresa Toby Travis Willy Y.C. Total 39 Neurodiversity paradigm Uses language that can be associated with a disorder narrative about autism Claims that the challenges faced by autistic individuals stem from autism Describes autistic traits with words that suggest deficit, delay, defect, excess Assumes normality exists and presents it as desirable Separates individuals into “normal people” and “people with autism” Conceives of autism as added on top of the “real person” Favors prevention, detection, cure, and/or amelioration of autism Defines/refers to autism as disorder and to autistic brains as abnormal, deviant, defective, and/or disordered Presents non-autistic health professionals, scientists, and other neurotypical individuals as the experts on autism Uses language that can be associated with a difference narrative about autism Claims that the challenges faced by autistic people stem from societal privileging of non-autistic brains Contextualizes, redefines, and or/revalorizes autistic traits Contextualizes, problematizes, and/or resists normality Separates individuals into autistic and non-autistic/ neurotypical people Conceives of autism as integral to the autistic person Favors accommodation, inclusion, and acceptance of autistic individuals Defines/refers to autism as a neurological difference and to autistic brains as different, distinct Presents autistic people as the experts on autism Medical model ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ 37 ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ 34 ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ 29 ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ 17 19 16 ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ 11 9 ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ 24 narrative about autism was the feature of the neurodiversity paradigm that the vlog entries exhibited most often. To illustrate the different language choices made by the vloggers, Becky states that “people with Asperger’s or Asperger tendencies are able to pretty well function in society.” Notice the use of person-first language and the description of levels of functioning established vis-à-vis a societal standard, both commonplace in the medical model ✓ 2 ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ 5 ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ ✓ 20 ✓ ✓ ✓ 3 5 10 6 3 1 of disability (Bagatell, 2010; Sinclair, 1999). In contrast, Angel’s language choices align with the neurodiversity paradigm when she asserts that “being autistic means that your brain functions differently.” She uses identity language (one is autistic, not a person with autism; Sinclair, 1999). Interestingly too, she compares autistic functioning to neurotypical functioning and describes the former as different: Some autistic individuals function differently, not Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism 581 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss well or poorly, when compared to the larger nonautistic group. Finally, in the preview to his video, Julio states: “I’m an aspie (…)11 a lot of people are having a hard time understanding what it’s like to have my disorder (…) this video will be on (…) what it means to have Asperger’s in the everyday experience.” In this case, note that the use of aspie as an identity is consistent with the neurodiversity paradigm, whereas the constructions of having a disorder and having Asperger’s are consistent with the medical model. Source of Challenges The source of the challenges faced by autistic individuals was not identified in 8% (3/39) of the videos. In the remainder 36 vlog entries, 87% (34/39) of the vloggers claimed that the challenges result directly from autism, making this feature of the medical model the second most frequently occurring in the videos; 13% (5/39) described the challenges as stemming from society’s privileging of neurotypical brains; and 8% (3/39) described the challenges as being both biological and societal in nature. To illustrate the attribution of challenges to autism, in his video, Rocco describes a hypothetical situation where an autistic “kid in class scoffs at an assignment or just completely tosses it to the side (…).” The vlogger explains the child’s behavior by stating that the child “has (…) Asperger’s syndrome, which very very much (…) puts his social understanding ability in a shit hole (…).” In other words, per the vlogger, the child’s difficulty to fulfill the given task results from his autistic lack of “social understanding,” although the same vlogger had mentioned previously that the child was “sick of doing the same thing over and over again,” thereby making boredom or tiredness a plausible alternative explanation. Conversely, Luis Angel accounts for his difficulties by referring to society’s failure to accommodate him: (…) I have an incredibly structured, organized, systematic, logical brain (…) the biggest problem that that presents is that most people…don’t have a brain like that. Unfortunately, the world is built to accommodate normal [makes air quotes]…people, and it’s not built to accommodate me (…) there’s lots of situations where [closes eyes and slightly shakes head] it’s extremely difficult for me, simply for the that fact I…don’t do things the same way as other people do things. Finally, representing a merger of the two approaches to the source of challenges, Angel attributes the difficulties faced by autistic people to both biologically given and socially constructed aspects. She states, “Autism is a different kind of brain (…) but it’s also a disability. This is because the autistic neurotype is often accompanied by impairments,” thus acknowledging a biological aspect. Later in the vlog, she comments, “It’s tough being autistic in a world that’s built for non-autistic people,” thereby recognizing a societal aspect to her disability. 11 As usual, (…) indicates that words were removed in the quote. 582 Description of Autistic Traits It was not possible to establish the specificity of the vlogger’s perception of autistic traits in 10% (4/39) of the videos. In the remaining vlog entries, 74% (29/39) of the vloggers described autistic traits with words that denote deficit, excess, delay, and/or defect. This was the third most frequently occurring feature of the medical model in the videos. The following are some examples: Deficit. “But, another issue I have with my autism is eye contact. I lack eye contact.” (Matt) Excess. “I would sort of like do [flails arms around] this a lot when I talk, or I would usually talk too loud.” (Nicole) Delay. “I still think I’m (…) a little bit socially delayed.” (Ken) Defect. “But I knew deep down in my heart (…) that sincerely something was different about me, something was wrong about me.” (Marcus) In contrast, 51% (20/39) of the vloggers contextualized, redefined, and/or revalorized autistic traits in their videos, making this the second most frequently occurring feature of the neurodiversity paradigm. Porter, for instance, both contextualizes and revalorizes the concept of selfstimulatory behaviors; she states, “As strange as it is, we hit ourselves because it’s pleasurable, but only when the world feels unpleasurable (…) we spin as an activity. Believe it or not, it helps stimulate our brains. It really does help us think.” Luis refers to autistic traits as “quirks” and states the following about his restless mind: (…) when I lay down my head at night (…) most people (…) can just go to sleep, turn their mind off, and rest (…) not me. That channel is still clicking. There are a hundred different stories going on my mind, and it’s darn near impossible to turn off my mind and just sleep because stories are going on over and over and over in my head (…) But (…) that’s the part of my creativity and, uh, the reason why I like to write, and where the ideas come from (…). Lastly, 36% (14/39) of the vloggers both (a) described autistic traits with words that suggest deficit, defect, delay, and/or excess and (b) contextualized, redefined, and/or revalorized such traits. Morton, for example, states in relation to specific interests: (…) everybody has different things they like, but a person with Asperger’s do it a bit too well (…) a lot of people with Asperger’s are obsessed, uh, not always a bad thing. My dad is a train driver an engineer, and he likes doing things with engines, which is a good thing; he’s a good engineer, I guess. The vlogger describes the so-called restricted interests as obsessions (excesses), but at the same time, he contextualizes and revalorizes these obsessions: They are not bad all the time; in fact, in the case of his father—who per the vlogger is autistic as well—they have probably helped him to become a good engineer. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss Table 7. Number of videos exhibiting each feature of the medical model and the neurodiversity paradigm. Topic Neurodiversity paradigm Number of videos Uses language that can be associated with a disorder narrative about autism Claims that the challenges faced by autistic individuals stem from autism 95% (37/39) 62% (24/39) 56% (22/39) 0 13% (5/39) 8% (3/39) 8% (3/39) Describes autistic traits with words that suggest deficit, delay, defect, and/or excess Assumes normality exists and presents it as desirable Separates individuals into “normal people” and “people with autism” Conceives of autism as added on top of the “real person” Prevention, detection, cure, and/or amelioration of autism 74% (29/39) Uses language that can be associated with a difference narrative about autism Claims that the challenges faced by autistic people stem from societal privileging of nonautistic brains Contextualizes, redefines, and/ or revalorizes autistic traits 51% (20/39) 36% (14/39) 10% (4/39) Contextualizes, problematizes, and/or resists normality Separates individuals into autistic and nonautistic/ neurotypical people Conceives of autism as integral to the autistic person Accommodation, inclusion, and acceptance of autistic individuals Defines/refers to autism as a neurological difference and to autistic brains as different, distinct Presents autistic people as the experts on autism 8% (3/39) 0 49% (19/39) 13% (5/39) 0 38% (15/39) 26% (10/39) 3% (1/39) 38% (15/39) 15% (6/39) 0 56% (22/39) 8% (3/39) 0 69% (27/39) 3% (1/39) 0 92% (36/39) Medical model Language Source of challenges Description of autistic traits Position toward the concept of normality Types of people Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism Number of videos Number of videos presenting both features simultaneously Conception of autism in relation to the person Preferred course of action Explicit definition of autism Allocation of autism expertise Defines/refers to autism as a disorder and to autistic brains as abnormal, deviant, defective, and/or disordered Presents nonautistic health professionals, scientists, and other neurotypical individuals as the experts on autism 87% (34/39) 44% (17/39) 49% (19/39) 38% (15/39) 28% (11/39) 23% (9/39) 5% (2/39) Number of videos presenting none of the features 583 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Position Toward Normality In relation to normality as a concept, 49% (19/39) of the vloggers did not make any statement from which their position could be inferred; 44% (17/39) of the vloggers appeared to assume the existence of normality and presented it as desirable. To illustrate, Jerry describes how, “through therapy,” his “high level of motivation,” and his mother’s and peers’ “determination and high will power,” he has been able to continuously “better” himself, breaking “barriers” erected by his autism and becoming “unsevere.” The vlogger concludes by reminding his autistic viewers that “(…) life has many challenges, regardless of how unsevere you can become” and that “there will always be light at the very end of the tunnel.” Conversely, 8% (3/39) of the vloggers contextualized, problematized, and/or resisted the concept of normality. An example of resistance comes from Dolly’s video in which she describes the process of coming to embrace her disability. She comments, “(…) since I’ve become more conscious of identifying as disabled and less upset about being disabled, (…) if I’m walking along, and I know that I’m walking differently from someone else, it’s like, ‘surprise! I guess I’m disabled, so I walk like I’m disabled. Shock!’ Not really big deal.” Then she adds, “(…) professionals always talk about socials skills, and they frame social skills as being, um, able to look normal, but, I mean, what I would call my social skills have vastly improved now that I don’t, um, care about that anymore (…).” Types of People In regard to referencing groups of people, 38% (15/39) of the vloggers did not refer to nonautistic people in their videos or used more generic words to refer to them, for example, “other people” and “most kids.” Forty-nine percent (19/39) of the vloggers separated people into “normal” people and “people with autism.” To illustrate, Jonathan defines a person who experiences Asperger’s syndrome as follows: “(…) it’s like a hybrid of like autism and just a normal person.” Nelson asserts, “You know, I kind of, like, blend in with the normal, but, the truth is, I am not normal.” On the contrary, 13% (5/39) of the vloggers used words that did not categorize people according to normalcy. For instance, although Angel and Arwen separate nonautistics from autistics, the words they use do not imply normality but typicality. Angel states, “When a little fetus is developing, it’s either going to be neurotypical or neuroatypical.” Arwen asserts, “Aspie brains work differently from neurotypical brains, which means that we can see solutions to problems that neurotypicals just don’t think of.” Conception of Autism in Relation to the Person The vlogger’s conception of autism in relation to the person could not be established in 38% (15/39) of the vlog entries. However, 38% (15/39) of the vloggers described autism as something added on top of the real person. Marcus, for instance, states, “I have this disability (…) It does affect me, but it does not affect me on who I am as a person. I am 584 a human being.” In contrast, 26% (10/39) of the vloggers depicted autism as integral to the person, making this the third most frequently occurring feature of the neurodiversity paradigm in the videos. For example, Matt states, “So, if there were a cure for my autism and ADD, I would not take it because it makes me who I am, and I don’t want to change that.” Finally, one vlogger (3%), Jonathan, talked about autism as both an addition to and an integral part of the individual. By the end of the vlog entry, he addresses his viewers directly saying, “Don’t let a title bring you down to, like, the definition of your life. Okay? That title isn’t you. I mean, it’s gonna be with you for like your whole life and such, but that’s not you. You are you. That’s just a part of you.” However, earlier in the video, he had stated, “It’s what we are, and, I feel like if I was offered a cure for my Asperger’s, I would turn that down like, ‘No way!’ I mean, my Asperger’s is me, and I would lose me.” Preferred Course of Action The vlogger’s preference in relation to prevention, detection, and cure or accommodation, inclusion, and acceptance was not readily inferable in 56% (22/39) of the vlog entries. Regarding this same topic, 28% (11/39) of the vloggers favored prevention, detection, and/or cure of autism in their videos. To illustrate, Willy, who does not believe that people are born autistic, shares his resolution to try to cure his Asperger’s syndrome; he states, “(…) I’m gonna continue to heal my body, and we’ll see if I can cure this, just like I was trying to cure my anxiety and depression; um, it’s no different.” On the contrary, 15% (6/39) of the vloggers argued for the accommodation, inclusion, and acceptance of autistic individuals. Angel, for instance, states: It’s tough being autistic in a world that’s built for non-autistic people, but that’s what accommodations are for. What accommodations do is level the playing field for people with disabilities because when you’re disabled, when you have impairments, you don’t have the same opportunities as non-disabled people, but accommodations can change that. Explicit Definition of Autism In relation to this content area, 69% (27/39) of the vloggers neither explicitly defined autism nor used any of the contrasting words to refer to it (e.g., disorder and difference). From the remaining vloggers, 23% (9/39) defined or referred to autism as a neurological disorder and to autistic brains as abnormal, deviant, defective, or disordered. Conversely, 8% (3/39) of the vloggers defined or referred to autism as a neurological difference and to autistic brains as different. From the former group, Jake defines “autism/ Asperger” as “a little disorder or some sort of thing that goes (…) inside you, and you can’t get it out. There’s no cure. It’s just yourself.” Angel, in contrast, defines autism as “(…) a different kind of brain (…) a different kind of brain wiring.” She comments, “Being autistic means that your brain functions differently. The structure of the brain may be different than non-autistic people’s brain, and the American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss different areas of the brain and how they talk to each other may be different as well.” Allocation of Autism Expertise Regarding autism expertise, 92% (36/39) of the vloggers did not address this topic in their videos. Two presented nonautistic health professionals as the experts on autism, and one highlighted that autism expertise can only be attained when you are autistic. In response to critiques that she does not look autistic enough, Arwen, for instance, addresses skeptics by highlighting the expertise of health professionals. She says, “It is…so ignorant…to assume through a 10-minute video… 3…that you know more about what it’s like in here [taps her head]…than all of the experts…that have…3…evaluated me, tested me…7…It’s ridiculous really.” In contrast, like the participants in other studies (Brownlow, 2010; Brownlow & O’Dell, 2006; Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2002), Ryan perceives autistic individuals as the only experts on autism; he argues: There are a lot of you that might think you understand what it is like to live with Asperger syndrome, but you really don’t (…) Just reading up on it, what it is, and asking a doctor for answers about it will never give you the full picture. You have to be someone with Asperger’s syndrome, such as myself, to really understand. In the following Discussion section, the study’s key findings are reviewed, associations to prior studies are drawn, and implications for both research and clinical practice are offered. Discussion Description of the Vloggers Most, but not all, of the vloggers in this study’s data can be described as White male adults who reported to experience Asperger’s syndrome and were likely from the United States. This profile is consistent with the profile of most autistic participants within research in general and within first-person perspective inquiry in particular (Blagrave, 2016; DePape & Lindsay, 2016; Gelbar, Smith, & Reichow, 2014; Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2017; Kirby et al., 2014; Mazurek, Engelhardt, & Clark, 2015). The larger presence of White males in the data may result from the reported, albeit sometimes contested, higher prevalence of autism spectrum conditions among males from this racial/ethnic group (Christensen et al., 2016). In addition, the format of the data might have been a factor in the larger number of vloggers who reported to experience Asperger’s syndrome: Online vlogs might be more accessible to individuals with this autistic profile. Finally, the inclusionary criterion that the videos had to be in English was likely responsible in part for the high prevalence of vloggers tied to the United States, as the United States is one of the largest Englishspeaking countries in the world (Masani, 2012). Regardless of the reasons behind the demographics of the vloggers, we wish to highlight, as other researchers have, the need for more inquiry focused on the experiences of individuals from underrepresented sectors of the autistic community, such as females, children and adolescents, members of non-White ethnic/racial groups, nonspeaking individuals, and people with autism spectrum diagnoses different from Asperger’s syndrome. What is known about autism comes mainly from White male adults who experience Asperger’s syndrome or, to say it in the words of some vloggers, “a high-functioning form of autism.” The call for more research analyzing the experiences of underrepresented groups is justified by the likely influence of intersectional identities on an individual’s narrative about autism, as well as by the implications for professional practices that may arise from such distinct narratives (Burkett, Morris, Manning-Courtney, Anthony, & Shambley-Ebron, 2015; Carrington, Templeton, & Papinczak, 2003; Cridland, Jones, Caputi, & Magee, 2014; Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2017; Jones et al., 2013). Online discussions and logs, personal websites, and autobiographical books prove useful and convenient data sources for researchers and clinicians who wish to obtain firsthand accounts of the condition from individuals belonging in unrepresented sectors of the autistic community. Representation of Autism There were three key findings in relation to the representation of autism within the videos. First and foremost was the prevalence of a hybrid narrative about autism among the vloggers (see Tables 6 and 7). This finding is consistent with the results of previous studies of autistic perspectives— including those reviewed earlier—according to which there is often a complex representation of autism among individuals on the spectrum that contrasts with the monolithic account of the condition that predominates within the scholarly literature and among researchers, professionals, and laypeople (Brownlow, 2010; Brownlow & O’Dell, 2006; Davidson, 2008; Donaldson et al., 2017; Gillespie-Lynch et al., 2017; Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2002; Jones et al., 2013; Kapp et al., 2013; McLaughlin & Rafferty, 2014; Pellicano & Stears, 2011). Given the predominance of the medical model within professional and media-based discourse, it is not particularly surprising that the second key finding was a trend toward more medical model features across most content areas. The trend toward more medical model features was most apparent in regard to the source of the challenges faced by autistic individuals. Most vloggers (34/39) attributed the struggles they had to autism, which is consistent with findings of previous studies (McLaughlin & Rafferty, 2014). Despite this, as in other studies (Madriaga, 2010), the identification of societal barriers was not missing in the videos, with five vloggers attributing at least some of the challenges they face to the privileging of nonautistic ways of functioning. Before the 1960s, with the emergence of the social model of disability, there was little—if any—consideration of the idea that the difficulties disabled individuals have could also stem from the environment and not only from their own impairments. The social model, though, more directly addressed physical disability than it did for other Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism 585 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss less visible disabilities such as autism (Goodley, 2001; McClimens, 2003). The emergence of the neurodiversity paradigm helped fill this gap, but as noted previously, this approach to the understanding of autism dates to the 1990s and is still much less popular than the ubiquitous medical model of disability, which could also help to explain the larger presence of medical model features in the videos in the data. Despite the trend toward more medical model features across content areas, the third key finding was the relatively high prevalence of neurodiversity paradigm features related to the content areas of (a) language and (b) description of autistic traits. In relation to language, 62% (24/39) of the vloggers used language associated with a difference (rather than disorder) perspective, including identity-related words and constructions and recent coinages (e.g., Aspie, neurotypical) that are embraced by the neurodiversity paradigm and can be considered part of a “community-building mechanism” (Bagatell, 2010, p. 39). Of interest, it was not unusual for the same video to include language that was consistent with the neurodiversity paradigm and language constructions that were indicative of the medical model. In fact, this overlap occurred in 56% (22/39) of the videos. It can be argued that medical-based constructions reflecting a deficit view of autism may be the “first language” that many autistic individuals and their families are exposed to, especially when exploring diagnoses and interacting with many professionals (Areheart, 2008; Bricher, 2000; GillespieLynch et al., 2017; Shyman, 2016). However, it is possible that their language becomes more diversified as their experiences widen and their identities develop. The vloggers’ language choices may be considered reflective of the changing language landscape and the diversifying narratives about autism within the autism/autistic community. In relation to the description of autism traits, 51% (20/39) of the vloggers presented a positive dimension of their autistic traits and/or contradicted their alleged pervasive nature by circumscribing them to specific contexts, a feature that can be associated with the neurodiversity paradigm (Bagatell, 2010; Strauss, 2013). As with language, it was not unusual for the same video to include descriptions of autistic traits as both impairments and constructive and/or contextualized abilities. For instance, the medical model’s “stereotyped, or repetitive motor movements, use of objects, or speech” (APA, 2013, “Neurodevelopmental Disorders”) is presented by some vloggers as “stimming,” an autistic activity that takes motor and vocal forms and helps them to calm down in stressing environments, to focus, and to think. Eye contact is described by some vloggers as painful, distressing, scary, and useless given their difficulties to read facial expressions. Despite this, they deny the alleged pervasive, negative nature of their “lack of eye contact,” arguing that they limit it to interactions with individuals they are close to and that not making eye contact helps them to focus on the ongoing conversation. Lastly, the vloggers describe their sensory experiences as challenging too, but they also present their “hypersensitivity” as an ability to see and hear things neurotypicals cannot see or 586 hear, especially when it comes to what some of them regard as their “special interests.” In fact, the co-occurrence of the contrasting features related with the description of autistic traits took place most often when the vloggers discussed their interests. Although some vloggers used language such as “obsessed” and “narrow” to talk about their interests, they also mentioned the positive side of these “obsessions”: triggering/ enhancing creativity, facilitating learning when academic content is presented within the framework provided by the special interest, and leading to academic and job niches. The vloggers’ claims about potential “flip sides” of their autistic traits are consistent with other studies of firstperson perspectives (Jones et al., 2003; McLaughlin & Rafferty, 2014) and supported by a growing body of literature that portrays autism as a condition where weakness and strengths converge, with “facility to processing” and “enhanced perception” among the latter (Pellicano & Stears, 2011, p. 275). Further support and related implications come from the decision of companies (e.g., SAP, a German computer software giant; The Federal Loan Mortgage Corporation; Exceptional Minds Studios; Ford; and Microsoft) to recruit autistic personnel and even modify their recruitment campaigns in their attempt to tap into autistic features whose positive dimension had been long overlooked (Austin & Pisano, 2017; Armstrong, 2010; Dador, 2017; Kelland, 2013; Strauss, 2013). Clinical Implications The results of this study point to at least three different clinical implications. First is the need to familiarize oneself with the varying views of autism held by autistic individuals, as their perspectives are an integral component of evidence-based practice (ASHA, 2005). Understanding the views of individuals on one’s caseload is an important place to start. Other valuable resources are first-person perspective research studies, autobiographical books authored by individuals on the spectrum, and, given the large presence of autistic people in the Internet, online outlets such as personal webpages, blogs, vlogs, discussion lists, and social networks. Given the prevalence of the medical model of autism within professional training, efforts to understand the neurodiversity paradigm might be particularly warranted. Specific resources for oneself, clients, or family members include Armstrong’s (2010) book Neurodiversity: Discovering the Extraordinary Gifts of Autism, ADHD, Dyslexia, and other Brain Difference; online logs such as Erin Human: Writing, Cartooning, and Neurodiversity Designs (https://erinhuman.com/); Rosie King’s TED talk How Autism Freed Me to be Myself (King, 2013); YouTube channels such as Autistic Genius (n.d.) and Neurowonderful (n.d.); and children’s books such as All Cats Have Asperger’s Syndrome (Hoopmann, 2006). A second related implication is to reflect on the way we as professionals talk about autism and to listen to how our clients and their families talk about autism. Language use both reflects and shapes perceptions (Brisenden, 1986). American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss For example, regarding the distinction between identityfirst versus person-first language, one possibility would be to listen for and even ask which construction an individual person prefers (i.e., one could ask, “Do you have a preference for the phrase person with autism versus autistic person?”). This question could serve as a gateway for discussing different views of autism using Table 3 as a reference or handout, as deemed useful. In accordance, professional organizations such as ASHA may want to reconsider guidelines that currently require the use of person-first language, opting instead for the more flexible guideline of using language that prioritizes what individuals prefer to be called (see American Psychological Association, 2010), or that more closely reflect the different positions within the autistic community (see Pellicano & Stears, 2011). Responding to clients and their families in ways that respect their values and priorities is specified by ASHA’s evidence-based practice guidelines (ASHA, 2004, 2005) and the American Psychological Association (2010). Third and finally, data from this study suggest the importance of exploring the potential positive dimensions of autistic traits, both in research and in clinical practice. Related to language use, phrases such as symptoms of autism can be said to homologize autism to a disease and potentially harm an individual’s sense of self-worth; conversely, characteristics associated with autism could be perceived as more neutral, less normative, and more descriptive. A general guideline is to listen to clients and to not make assumptions about how they may perceive autistic traits. Clinicians may also want to consult with clients and/or their families regarding which autistic traits, for example, special interests, could be tapped into to facilitate participation and learning. Such information could then be used to develop supportive services and actions. As an example from the present data set, John described how over the years his perception of his interest in performing evolved from one of an obstacle to socialization to one of a catapult to finding his “purpose” in life. In such a case, an SLP might assist by supporting an individual’s participation in a drama club or developing a lunch group to support social interaction while playing charades. Limitations and Directions for Future Research In closing, we would like to acknowledge three potential limitations of our study and to suggest related directions for future narrative inquiry into the experience of autism. First and foremost, the data here do not and were not intended to capture the full diversity of views and perspectives from autistic individuals. In addition to limited diversity related to specified demographics (e.g., race/ ethnicity), this study might have failed to represent more introverted individuals who are less inclined to publish videos about their life on the spectrum (Guadagno, Okdie, & Eno, 2008; L. Jones & Holland, 2013). Forthcoming narrative studies can increase representation of the different sectors of the autistic community by analyzing narratives presented in other formats (e.g., written weblogs, autobiographies, drawings) and by purposefully sampling underrepresented demographics (e.g., autistic individuals of color). A second point for consideration relates to the varied lengths across videos studied, which could lead to some videos being more influential in the analysis than others. Consistent with the practice of narrative inquiry (Polkinghorne, 1988), there was no attempt to control the length or specified content of the videos. The study intentionally focused on all content provided at the discretion of the vloggers, which meant that some vloggers chose to cover more topics of interest than others. Of import, the length of the video was not directly associated with the density of features. For example, the longest video, authored by Teresa, exhibited only two features of the medical model and two features of the neurodiversity paradigm, despite being longer than 39 min, whereas the shortest video, authored by Porter, exhibited four features of the medical model and two features of the neurodiversity paradigm (see Tables 1 and 6). However, future research using a different methodology might consider developing a survey from the 18 contrastive features outlined in Tables 3 and 4 to directly give each participant an opportunity to respond to each topic of interest. Such a measure might also prove useful in clinical work, as a way to directly assess individuals’ and/or their caregivers’ view toward autism. Lastly, future narrative inquiry could build on the study reported here by analyzing videos posted after 2015. Given the recent rise of the neurodiversity paradigm, one might anticipate a great density of paradigm features in later years. Although the data in this study did not appear to support that assumption within the time frame studied, the study of the evolution in the representation of autism over time and across different cultural-linguistic landscapes certainly merits further attention. Acknowledgments We wish to thank Marie Channell and Meghan Burke, who served as members of the advising committee on this project; our external consultant, Scott Robertson; and the research assistants involved in this study: Grace Lee, Lauren De Vries, Stephanie Cheng, Mindy Eng, and Amanda Moy. Special thanks to research assistants Michelle Chan, Regan Kelley, and McKenzie Kamen for being with us from the beginning of this project to its culmination. References Alleyne, R. (2008). YouTube: Overnight success has sparked a backlash. The Telegraph. Retrieved from https://www.telegraph. co.uk/news/uknews/2480280/YouTube-Overnight-success-hassparked-a-backlash.html American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5 (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author. American Psychological Association. (2010). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism 587 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2004). Report of the joint coordinating committee on evidence-based practice (pp. 1–20). Rockville, MD: Author. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2005). Evidencebased practice in communication disorders [Position statement]. Retrieved from http://www.asha.org/policy American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2010). DSM-V development ASHA comments submitted April 2010. Retrieved from https://www.asha.org/uploadedFiles/April-2010-DSM-VDevelopment-Comments.pdf American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2011). DSM-V development ASHA comments submitted June 2011. Retrieved from https://www.asha.org/uploadedFiles/June-2011-DSM-VDevelopment-Comments.pdf American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2012a). ASHA’s recommended revisions to the DSM-5. Retrieved from https:// www.asha.org/uploadedFiles/DSM-5-Final-Comments.pdf American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2012b). ASHA letter sent June 2012. Retrieved from https://www.asha.org/ uploadedFiles/DSM-Final-Letter.pdf American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (n.d.). Autism. Retrieved from https://www.asha.org/PRPSpecificTopic.aspx? folderid=8589935303&section=Overview Areheart, B. A. (2008). When disability isn’t “just right”: The entrenchment of the medical model of disability and the goldilocks dilemma. Indiana Law Journal, 1(83), 181–232. Armstrong, T. (2010). Neurodiversity: Discovering the extraordinary gifts of autism, ADHD, dyslexia, and other brain differences (1st ed.). Boston, MA: Da Capo Lifelong Books. Aronson, J. (1995). A pragmatic view of thematic analysis. The Qualitative Report, 2(1), 1–3. Austin, R. D., & Pisano, G. P. (2017). Neurodiversity as a competitive advantage. Harvard Business Review, 95(3), 96–103. Autistic Genius. (n.d.). Home [YouTube channel]. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/user/Fagin110 Autistic Self-Advocacy Network. (2016). Nothing about us without us. Retrieved from https://autisticadvocacy.org/home Bagatell, N. (2010). From cure to community: Transforming notions of autism. Journal of the Society for Psychological Anthropology, 38(1), 33–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1548-1352. 2009.01080.x Blagrave, A. J. (2016). Experiences of middle school children with autism spectrum disorders in adapted physical education (Doctoral dissertation). College of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Walden University, Minneapolis, MN. Boyatzis, R. E. (1998). Transforming qualitative information: Thematic analysis and code development. Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications. Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77–101. Bricher, G. (2000). Disabled people, health professionals and the social model of disability: Can there be a research relationship? Disability and Society, 5(5), 781–793. Brisenden, S. (1986). Independent living and the medical model of disability. Disability, Handicap, and Society, 1(2), 173–178. https://doi.org/10.1080/02674648666780171 Brownlow, C. (2010). Re-presenting autism: The construction of ‘NT syndrome’. Journal of Medical Humanities, 31, 243–255. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10912-010-9114-4 Brownlow, C., & O’Dell, L. (2006). Constructing an autistic identity: AS Voices Online. Mental Retardation, 44(5), 315–321. Burkett, K. K., Morris, E., Manning-Courtney, P., Anthony, J., & Shambley-Ebron, D. (2015). African American families on autism diagnosis and treatment: The influence of culture. 588 Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(10), 3244–3254. Carrington, S., Templeton, E., & Papinczak, T. (2003). Adolescents with Asperger syndrome and perceptions of friendship. Focus on Autism & Other Developmental Disabilities, 18(4), 211–218. https://doi.org/10.1177/10883576030180040201 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017). Autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/ autism/data.html Chamak, B., Bonniau, B., Jaunay, E., & Cohen, D. (2008). What can we learn about autism from autistic persons? Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 77, 271–279. https://doi.org/10.1159/ 000140086 Christensen, D. L., Baio, J., Braun, K. V. N., Bilder, D., Charles, J., Constantino, J. N., . . . Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2016). Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2012. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 65(No. SS-3), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6503a1 Cortazzi, M. (1993). Narrative analysis (12). London, United Kingdom: Falmer Press. Cridland, E., Jones, S., Caputi, P., & Magee, C. (2014). Being a girl in a boys’ world: Investigating the experiences of girls with autism spectrum disorders during adolescence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(6), 1261–1274. Dador, D. (2017). Young visual artists with autism nominated for Oscar for ‘Doctor Strange.’ ABC7. Retrieved from https:// abc7.com/1772351/ Davidson, J. (2008). Autistic culture online: Virtual communication and cultural expression on the spectrum. Social & Cultural Geography, 9(7), 791–806. https://doi.org/10.1080/14649360802382586 Davis, L. J. (2013). Introduction: Normality, power, and culture. In L. J. Davis (Ed.), The disability reader (4th ed., pp. 1–14). New York, NY: Routledge. DePape, A. M., & Lindsay, S. (2016). Lived experiences from the perspective of individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A qualitative meta-synthesis. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 31(1), 60–71. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1088357615587504 Dewsbury, G., Clarke, K., Dave, R., Rouncefield, M., & Sommerville, I. (2004). The anti-social model of disability. Disability and Society, 19(2), 145–158. https://doi.org/10.1080/0968759042000181776 Donaldson, A., Krejcha, K., & McMillin, A. (2017). A strengthsbased approach to autism: Neurodiversity and partnering with the autism community. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 2(1), 56–68. Endow, J. (n.d.). Ollibean. Retrieved from https://ollibean.com/ the-pseudo-logic-of-not-autistic-enough/ Fereday, J., & Muir-Cochrane, E. (2006). Demonstrating rigor using thematic analysis: A hybrid approach of inductive and deductive coding and theme development. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 5(1), 80–92. Gelbar, N. G., Smith, I., & Reichow, B. (2014). Systematic review of articles describing experience and supports of individuals with autism enrolled in college and university programs. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(10), 2593–2601. Giangreco, M. F. (2004). “The stairs didn’t go anywhere!”: A selfadvocate’s reflections on specialized services and their impact on people with disabilities. In M. Nind, K. Sheehy, J. Rix, & K. Simmons (Eds.), Inclusive education: Diverse perspectives (pp. 32–42). London, United Kingdom: David Fulton Publishers in association with The Open University. Gillespie-Lynch, K., Kapp, S., Brooks, P., Pickens, J., & Schwartzman, B. (2017). Whose expertise is it? Evidence for autistic adults as American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss critical autism experts. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–14. https:// doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00438 Goering, S. (2015). Rethinking disability: The social model of disability and chronic disease. Current Reviews in Musculoskeletal Medicine, 8(2), 134–138. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12178-015-9273-z Goodley, D. (2001). “Learning difficulties”: The social model of disability and impairment: Challenging epistemologies. Disability and Society, 16(2), 207–231. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 09687590120035816 Grant, D. (2009). The psychological assessment of neurodiversity. In D. Pollak (Ed.), Neurodiversity in higher education: Positive responses to specific learning differences (pp. 33–62). United Kingdom: Wiley-Blackwell. Guadagno, R. E., Okdie, B. M., & Eno, C. A. (2008). Who blogs? Personality predictors of blogging. Computers in Human Behavior, 24(5), 1993–2004. Guest, G., MacQueen, K. M., & Namey, E. E. (2012). Applied thematic analysis. Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications. Haegele, J. A., & Hodge, S. (2016). Disability discourse: Overview and critiques of the medical and social models. Quest, 68(2), 193–206. https://doi.org/10.1080/00336297.2016.1143849 Hayasaki, E. (2015). The debate over an autism cure turns hostile. Newsweek. Retrieved from https://www.newsweek.com/2015/02/ 27/one-activists-search-cure-his-autism-drawing-violent-backlash306998.html Healy, S., Msetfi, R., & Gallagher, S. (2013). ‘Happy and a bit nervous’: The experiences of children with autism in physical education. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 222–228. https://doi.org/10.1111/bld.12053 Hengst, J. A. (2003). Collaborative referencing between individuals with aphasia and routine communication partners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(4), 831–848. https:// doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/065) Holloway, I., & Freshwater, D. (2007). Narrative research in nursing. Oxford: Blackwell. Holtgrave, D. (2015). Broadcasting the stigmatized self: Positioning functions of youtube vlogs on bipolar disorder. In T. J. Blank & A. Kitta (Eds.), Diagnosing folklore perspectives on disability, health, and trauma (pp. 175–193). Jackson: University Press of Mississippi. Hoopmann, K. (2006). All cats have autism (1st ed.). London, United Kingdom: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Howard, B., Cohn, E., & Orsmond, G. I. (2006). Understanding and negotiating friendships perspectives from an adolescent with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 10(6), 619–627. https://doi.org/10. 1177/1362361306068508 Humphrey, N., & Lewis, S. (2008). ‘Make me normal’: The views and experiences of pupils on the autistic spectrum in mainstream secondary schools. Autism, 12(1), 23–46. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1362361307085267 Hurlbutt, K., & Chalmers, L. (2002). Adults with autism speak out: Perceptions of their life experiences. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 17(2), 103–111. Hurlbutt, K., & Chalmers, L. (2004). Employment and adults with asperger syndrome. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 19(4), 215–222. Hurwitz, B., Greenhalgh, T., & Skultans, V. (2004). Narrative research in health and illness. Malden, MA: BMJ Books. Huws, J. C., & Jones, R. S. P. (2008). Diagnosis, disclosure, and having autism: An interpretative phenomenological analysis of the perceptions of young people with autism. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 33(2), 99–107. https://doi. org/10.1080/13668250802010394 Jaarsma, P., & Welin, S. (2012). Autism as a natural variation: Reflections on the claims of the neurodiversity movement. Health Care Analysis, 20, 20–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10728-011-0169-9 Jones, L., & Holland, A. (2013). Who blogs? Understanding the correlation of personality and blogging in cultural discussions. CALICO Journal, 30, 92–117. Retrieved from https://www. jstor.org/stable/calicojournal.30.92 Jones, R. S. P., Huws, J., & Beck, G. (2013). ‘I’m not the only person out there’: Insider and outsider understandings of autism. International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 59(2), 134–144. https://doi.org/10.1179/2047387712Y.0000000007 Jones, R. S. P., Quigney, C., & Huws, J. (2003). First-hand accounts of sensory perceptual experiences in autism: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 28(2), 112–121. https://doi.org/10.1080/1366825031000147058 Jones, R. S. P., Zahl, A., & Huws, J. C. (2001). First-hand accounts of emotional experiences in autism: A qualitative analysis. Disability & Society, 16(3), 393–401. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 09687590120045950 Kapp, S., Gillespie-Lynch, K., Sherman, L., & Hutman, T. (2013). Deficit, difference, or both? Autism and neurodiversity. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 59–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/ a0028353 Kelland, K. (2013). Thinking differently: Autism finds space in the workplace. Reuters. Retrieved from https://www.reuters.com/ article/us-autism-recruitment-idUSBRE9530KA20130604 King, R. (2013). How autism freed me to be myself/Rosie King [Video file]. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/watch? v=jQ95xlZeHo8&t=21s Kirby, A. V., Dickie, V. A., & Baranek, G. T. (2014). Sensory experiences of children with autism spectrum disorder: In their own words. Autism, 19(3), 316–326. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1362361314520756 Levitt, H. M., Bamberg, M., Creswell, J. W., Josselson, R., Frost, D. M., & Suarez-Orozco, C. (2018). Journal article reporting standards for qualitative primary, qualitative meta-analytic, and mixed methods research in psychology: The APA publications and communications board task force report. American Psychologist, 73(1), 26–46. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa. org/record/2018-00750-003 Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications. Madriaga, M. (2010). ‘I avoid pubs and the student union like the plague’: Students with Asperger syndrome and their negotiation of university spaces. Children’s Geographies, 8(1), 23–34. https://doi.org/10.1080/14733280903500166 Masani, Z. (2012). English or Hinglish—Which will India choose? BBC News. Retrieved from https://www.bbc.com/news/magazine20500312 Mazurek, M. O., Engelhardt, C. R., & Clark, K. E. (2015). Video games from the perspective of adults with autism spectrum disorder. Computers in Human Behavior, 51, 122–130. McClimens, A. (2003). The organization of difference: People with intellectual disabilities and the social model of disability. Mental Retardation, 41(1), 35–46. McLaughlin, S., & Rafferty, H. (2014). Me and ‘It’: Seven young people given a diagnosis of Asperger’s syndrome. Educational & Child Psychology, 31(1), 63–78. Miller, J. F., & Long, S. (2005). Language sample analysis II: the Wisconsin guide (Rev.). Madison, WI: Wisconsin Department of Public Instruction. Monje, M. S., Jr. (2015). Not that autistic. In The real experts (1st ed., pp. 61–68). Fort Worth, TX: Autonomous Press. Angulo-Jiménez & DeThorne: Narratives About Autism 589 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss Muller, E., Schuler, A., & Yates, G. B. (2008). Social challenges and supports from the perspective of individuals with Asperger syndrome and other autism spectrum disabilities. Autism, 12(2), 173–190. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361307086664 Neurowonderful. (n.d.). Home [YouTube channel]. Retrieved from https://www.youtube.com/user/neurowonderful Oakley, B. (2012). The word I choose [Snagglebox]. Retrieved from https://www.snagglebox.com/article/the-word-autistic Parasole, S. M. (2015). Discourses of disability, narratives of community: Reclaiming an autistic identity online. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 43(3), 336–356. https://doi. org/10.1080/00909882.2015.1052829 Paul, D. (2013). Yes, DSM-5 changes SLP-relevant disorder categories: What you need to know. Retrieved from https://blog. asha.org/2013/06/18/yes-dsm-5-changes-up-communicationdisorder-categories-what-you-need-to-know/ Peers, D., Spencer-Cavaliere, N., & Eales, L. (2014). Say what you mean: Rethinking disability language in adapted physical activity quarterly. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 31, 265–282. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1123/apaq.20130091 Pellicano, E., Dinsmore, A., & Charman, T. (2014). What should autism research focus upon? Community views and priorities from the United Kingdom. Autism, 18(7), 756–770. https://doi. org/10.1177/1362361314529627 Pellicano, E., & Stears, M. (2011). Bridging autism, science and society: Moving toward an ethically informed approach to autism research. Autism Research, 4, 271–282. https://doi.org/10.1002/ aur.201 Polkinghorne, D. E. (1988). Narrative knowing and the human sciences. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Polkinghorne, D. E. (1995). Narrative configuration in qualitative analysis. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 8(1), 5–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/0951839950080103 Polkinghorne, D. E. (2005). Narrative psychology and historical consciousness: Relationships and perspectives. In J. Straub (Ed.), Narration identity and historical consciousness (Vol. 3, pp. 3–22). New York, NY: Berghan Books. Pollak, D. (2009). Introduction. In Neurodiversity in higher education: Positive responses to specific learning differences (pp. 1–11). United Kingdom: Wiley-Blackwell. Preece, D., & Jordan, R. (2009). Obtaining the views of children and young people with autism spectrum disorders about their experience of daily life and social care support. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38, 10–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1468-3156.2009.00548.x Robertson, R. (2011). Sharing stories: Motherhood, autism, and culture. In C. Lewiecki-Wilson & J. Cellio (Eds.), Disability and mothering: Liminal spaces of embodied knowledge (1st ed., pp. 140–155). NY: University Press. Sanchez-Cortes, D., Kumano, S., Otsuka, K., & Gatica-Perez, D. (2015). In the mood for vlog: Multimodal inference in conversational social video. ACM Transactions on Interactive Intelligent Systems, 5(2), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1145/2641577 Scientific Software Development GmbH. (n.d.). What is Atlas.ti? Retrieved from https://atlasti.com/product/what-is-atlas-ti/ Shakespeare, T. (2013). The social model of disability. The disability studies reader (4th ed., pp. 460–484). New York: Routledge. 590 Sharpened Productions. (n.d.). Vlog. Tech terms—The tech terms computer dictionary. Retrieved from https://techterms.com/ definition/vlog Shyman, E. (2016). The reinforcement of ableism: Normality, the medical model of disability, and humanism in applied behavior analysis and ASD. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 54(5), 366–376. https://doi.org/10.1352/1934-9556-54.5.366 Silberman, S. (2015). Neurotribes: The legacy of autism and the future of neurodiversity. New York, NY: Avery, an Imprint of Penguin Random House. Silvers, A. (2009). An essay on modeling: The social model of disability. In Philosophical reflections on disability, philosophy and medicine (Vol. 104, pp. 19–36). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer. Simmons-Mackie, N., & Damico, J. (1999). Social role negotiation in aphasia therapy: Competence, incompetence and conflict. In Constructing (In)competence: Disabling evaluations in clinical and social interaction (1st ed.). New York: Psychology Press. Sinclair, J. (1999). Why I dislike “person-first” language. Retrieved from https://autismmythbusters.com/general-public/autistic-vspeople-with-autism/jim-sinclair-why-i-dislike-person-first-language/ Sinclair, J. (2010). Being autistic together. Disability Studies Quarterly, 30(1). Retrieved from http://dsq-sds.org/article/view/1075/1248 Sinclair, J. (2013). Why I dislike “person first” language. Autonomy, the Critical Journal of Interdisciplinary Autism Studies, 1(2). Retrieved from https://www.larry-arnold.net/Autonomy/index. php/autonomy/article/view/OP1/pdf Sisti, D. (2015). Naturalism and the social model of disability: Allied or antithetical? Journal of Medical Ethics, 41, 553–556. https://doi.org/10.1136/medethics-2014-102127 Squire, C., Andrews, M., & Tamboukou, M. (2013). Introduction: What is narrative research? In M. Andrews, C. Squire & M. Tamboukou (Eds.), Doing narrative research (2nd ed., pp. 1–26). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. Strauss, J. (2013). Autism as culture. In The disability reader (4th ed., pp. 460–484). New York, NY: Routledge. Teijeiro-Mosquera, L., Biel, J.-I., Alba-Castro, J. L., & Gatica-Perez, D. (2015). What your face vlogs about: Expressions of emotion and big-five traits impressions in YouTube. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 6(2), 193–205. https://doi.org/10.1109/ TAFFC.2014.2370044 Walker, N. (2013). Throw away the master’s tools: Liberating ourselves from the pathology paradigm. Retrieved from https:// neurocosmopolitanism.com/throw-away-the-masters-toolsliberating-ourselves-from-the-pathology-paradigm/ Walker, N. (2014a). Neurodiversity: Some basic terms & definitions. Retrieved from https://neurocosmopolitanism.com/neurodiversitysome-basic-terms-definitions/ Walker, N. (2014b). Advice to young autistics: Stick around and be awesome. Retrieved from https://neurocosmopolitanism.com/ advice-to-young-autistics-stick-around-and-be-awesome/ Watermeyer, B., & Kathard, H. (2016). To be or not to be: Stuttering and the human costs of being “un-disabled.” International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 18(1), 11–19. https://doi. org/10.3109/17549507.2015.1060528 Webster, L., & Mertova, P. (2007). Using narrative inquiry as a research method: An introduction to using critical event narrative analysis in research on learning and teaching. London, United Kingdom: Routledge. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 28 • 569–590 • May 2019 wnloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org University Library at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign on 05/29/2019, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permiss