Subido por Nastia Grijalva

Korean Deaf Community

Anuncio
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education
Korean Deaf Adolescents’ Awareness of Thematic and
Taxonomic Relations among Ordinary Concepts Represented
by Pictures and Written Words
Kwangoh Yi1, Degao Li*,2, Woon Seok Park1, Kyung-hee Park3, Tae-taek Shim4,
Ohgoo Kwern5, Jung Yeon Kim1
Yeungnam University
2
Zhejiang University
Yeungnam Middle School
3
4
5
Daegu Younghwa School for the Deaf
Yeounil Elementary School
Received July 1, 2010; revisions received December 5, 2010; accepted December 7, 2010
Individuals’ relative awareness of thematic and taxonomic
relations is influenced by factors such as language and background knowledge. Relatively weak in Korean language skills
and also having relatively limited social opportunities,
Korean deaf adolescents might be different from hearing
adolescents in how they make decisions in taxonomically
and thematically associated entities represented by pictures
and words. Experiment 1 indicated that deaf adolescents had
longer reaction times than hearing adolescents in a forcedchoice decision-making task. Both deaf and hearing adolescents had shorter reaction times and higher accuracies with
pictures than with words, but deaf adolescents’ differences
were bigger than those of hearing adolescents. Experiment
2 further showed that deaf adolescents had lower accuracies
than hearing adolescents in a priming task of living–nonliving categorization. Both deaf and hearing adolescents
had shorter reaction times with taxonomic than with
thematic categories, but deaf adolescents’ difference was
bigger than that of hearing adolescents. In conclusion,
Korean deaf adolescents were aware of thematic and taxonomic relations less than hearing adolescents in general.
They were more likely than hearing adolescents to show
the advantage of pictures over words in their performance
in conceptual activities and to prefer taxonomic to thematic
associations for written words in Experiment 2.
*Correspondence should be sent to Degao Li, School of International
Studies, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China (e-mail: lidegao@hzcnc.
com).
Taxonomic and Thematic Categories
Taxonomic categories are categories that are organized
in a hierarchical system based on similarities among
their exemplars (Rosch, Mervis, Gray, Johnson, &
Boyes-Braem, 1976). Basic-level categories are more
similar and share more properties than categories of
a super-ordinate level. For example, properties true
to animals, such as living and breathing, are true also
to birds, but properties true to birds, such as flying and
having a beak, are not shared by all other animals. This
structure determines the taxonomic relationships between categories of the same level and between categories of different levels. Actually, exemplars in the
same category are differentiated from one another by
the properties they have (for review, see Jerger &
Damian, 2005). Those that have a high frequency of
characteristic properties are called typical exemplars.
Individuals tend to agree on which exemplars of
a category are the most typical, and typical ones are
easier to retrieve from memory (Gastgeb, Strauss, &
Minshew, 2006), more representative of the category,
and share more similarities than less typical exemplars.
Basic-level categories have the clearest boundaries, are
expressed in the simplest linguistic forms, and are
more consistent across different cultures than those
Ó The Author 2011. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.
For Permissions, please email: [email protected]
doi:10.1093/deafed/enq065
Advance Access publication on February 16, 2011
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
1
376
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16:3 Summer 2011
were biased toward the thematic association because
their knowledge of the world was mainly based on life
experiences (Scheuner, Bonthoux, Cannard, & Blaye,
2004). In another study, also elderly people tended to
prefer thematic to taxonomic relations in categorization tasks in comparison with young adults because
autobiographical experiences constrain the perception
of commonalities, and conceptual preferences are
modified as a result (Pennequin et al., 2006).
Deaf individuals can be different from hearing
individuals in many ways. For example, living in the
same cultural environment as hearing individuals, deaf
individuals born to hearing parents are likely to be at
the risk of having fewer language experiences and/
or receiving less education than their hearing peers
(Marschark, Lang, & Albertini, 2002). Thus, one
might wonder whether deaf individuals can be different from hearing individuals in their relative awareness of taxonomic and thematic associations among
ordinary entities.
Deaf Individuals’ Conceptual Organizations
Young deaf children do not seem to be different from
young hearing children in their conceptual development in general. For example, Blank and Bridger
(1966) tested 3- and 5-year-old hearing and deaf children on problems involving the concepts of something
versus nothing (e.g., light vs. no light) to compare
their ability to transfer a concept across sensory modalities. The results showed that the 3-year-old deaf
and hearing children performed equally poorly and
that the 5-year-old deaf and hearing children performed equally well, indicating that young deaf and
hearing children are equally able to have their concepts developed.
Motivated by the fact that deaf students lag behind
hearing students at mathematics, studies were conducted into deaf children’s representations of numbers. For example, 3- and 4-year-old deaf and
hearing children had the same ability to remember
and to reproduce the number of items in sets of
objects, suggesting that preschool deaf children’s
number representation is as advanced as that of hearing children (Zarfaty, Nunes, & Bryant, 2004).
Utterances expressing generic kinds (e.g., birds fly
and wheels go around) refer to categories of objects
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
of other levels. The taxonomic relationships between
basic-level categories of high typicality are stronger
than between those of low typicality.
Thematically associated items can also be taken as
categories, whose exemplars are associated by external
relations, such as part-whole (e.g., a roof is on top of
a house), functional (e.g., a piece of chalk is used to
write on a blackboard), causal (e.g., electricity makes
a light bulb glow), and temporal (e.g., bills typically
come after meals in restaurants) relations among
objects, events, people, and other entities that interact
(Lin & Murphy, 2001). Mandler (1977, p. 263) took
thematic categories as ‘‘parts that are connected on the
basis of contiguities that have been experienced in
space or time.’’ In a thematic category, the exemplars
have different but complementary roles in the same
scene or event.
Thematic associations emphasize events rather than
taxonomic similarities (Markman & Hutchinson, 1984).
The development of taxonomic knowledge can be
effectively fostered by communication and education
(Vygotsky, 1999), but knowledge of thematic associations is mainly based on life experiences. For instance,
the word rain can be expected to activate more concepts
for Londoners than for Tibetans. A toddler may put
a toy car and a toy person together and say, ‘‘A person
drives a car.’’ For Koreans ‘‘horse’’ is closely associated
with ‘‘carrot’’ because a horse eats a carrot in a traditional Korean story, which almost every Korean has
heard hundreds of times since he or she was young.
Thematically associated items can be characterized by
the familiarity of the items and by the strength of associations between them (e.g., Pennequin, Fontaine,
Bonthoux, Scheuner, & Blaye, 2006).
Thematic and taxonomic relations coexist in the
conceptual organizations of human beings (Blaye
& Bonthoux, 2001; Blewitt & Toppino, 1991; Borghi
& Caramelli, 2003; Lin & Murphy, 2001; Markman &
Hutchinson, 1984). Because of the influences of factors such as language and background knowledge,
whether taxonomic relations are relatively more salient
than thematic associations or vice versa can be different for different people. Suppose Picture A was thematically related to Picture B and taxonomically
related to Picture C. When choosing which of B and
C went well with A, 4- and 6-year-old hearing children
Korean Deaf Adolescents’ Awareness of Thematic and Taxonomic Relations 377
that college deaf and hearing students were similar
in their overall organization of lexical knowledge, but
hearing students had stronger associations than deaf
students between category names and exemplar words
(Marschark, Convertino, McEvoy, & Masteller, 2004).
In addition, college deaf students had relatively stronger associations from words for taxonomic categories
of basic level (e.g., snake) to those of super-ordinate
level (e.g., reptiles) than vice versa, in comparison with
hearing students.
Li and Zhang (2009) required deaf and hearing
adolescents to memorize concrete words and pictures
for exemplars of taxonomic, thematic, and slot-filler
(Lucariello & Nelson, 1985) categories and then recall
and write on pieces of paper as many words for the
categories as possible. Slot-filler categories (e.g., lunch
foods) are categories that are based on events (e.g.,
eaten at lunch), the actions of which are complemented
by concrete things (e.g., steamed bread, rice or salted
fish). The results showed the deaf adolescents had
lower ratios of correct recall of stimuli than the hearing adolescents. The deaf adolescents seemed aware of
relationships between entities less than the hearing
adolescents in the memory task. In another study, deaf
adolescents performed two tasks: (a) deciding whether
four sequentially presented basic-level exemplars were
of the same taxonomic category and (b) studying some
words for basic-level exemplars and then recognizing
both the studied and some nonstudied words for
exemplars of the same categories (Zhang, Li, & Wu,
2008). The deaf adolescents’ performance was poorer
with exemplars of low typicality than with those of
high typicality. They were certain about which categories the taxonomic exemplars of high typicality
belong to but seemed less certain with those of low
typicality, in comparison with the hearing adolescents.
To examine the quality of the semantic categorization
of deaf children (age range: 8.6–12.4 years), Ormel
et al. (2010) used basic-level exemplars as stimuli
and conducted a forced-choice task: The participants
had to select one item (e.g., mosquito) that best
matched the target (e.g., bees) from the four simultaneously presented items in a trial. The deaf children
tended to have a lower accuracy than their hearing
counterparts, and the authors concluded that the
semantic–categorical knowledge of deaf children
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
(birds and wheels as classes of objects). Birds fly
implies a property that applies to birds in general.
Goldin-Meadow, Gelman, and Mylander (2005)
observed that young deaf children from America and
China, who were born to hearing parents and not
exposed to a conventional sign language, used
generics in the home signs they invented to express
generic kinds. For example, the deaf American
children first pointed at a picture of an unmoving
pinwheel, then at an unconnected wheel, and made
a go-around gesture to express wheels go around. The
researchers found that the young deaf American and
Chinese children produced generic utterances at
about the same rate as the hearing children growing
up in the same cultures, suggesting that young deaf
children are similar to hearing children in their
categorical knowledge.
Similarly, second-generation deaf children seemed
to have the same pattern of relative awareness of thematic and taxonomic relations as hearing children of
the same age (Courtin, 1997). To examine how sign
language affected 6-year-old deaf children’s categorization of ordinary entities, Courtin required deaf and
hearing children to select thematic or taxonomic alternatives as associations to the target in a trial: Either
choosing one from a pair of items that were thematically associated with the target or one from a pair of
items that were taxonomically related to the target.
The materials were presented in two conditions: linguistic condition in which the three taxonomically related items shared a generic sign in sign language and
control condition in which the three taxonomically
related items did not share a common generic sign.
The deaf children, who were native sign language
users, were more likely than the hearing children to
prefer taxonomic to thematic relations under the linguistic condition. When the influence of sign language
was eliminated under the control condition, however,
they were not different from the hearing children
in their performance of selection. Like the hearing
children, the deaf children preferred thematic to taxonomic choices.
Deaf adults and adolescents, however, seem different from hearing adults and adolescents, respectively,
in experimental performance. For example, the primary responses in a word association task revealed
378
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16:3 Summer 2011
Hypothesis
Conceptual organizations are usually investigated with
written words as stimuli, but deaf individuals are generally weaker at written languages than hearing individuals. For example, elementary school (Monreal &
Hernández, 2005), high school (Kyle & Harris, 2006),
and college (Gaustad, Kelly, Payne, & Lylak, 2002)
deaf students’ reading development can be seriously
delayed. Deaf individuals’ age of acquisition of vocabulary was later than that of hearing individuals (Auer
& Bernstein, 2008). The result of a vocabulary test to
10-, 11-, and 12-year-old deaf children showed that
they found it extremely difficult to master the second
meaning of a multimeaning word (Paul, 1987). The
ability of preschool deaf children to learn novel words
from context was weaker than that of hearing children
of the same age (Lederberg & Spencer, 2009). High
school deaf students’ literate proficiency usually cannot surpass that of elementary school hearing students
of Grade 4 (Harris & Moreno, 2004).
How to recognize written words is a learned skill
that is developed several years later than how to
recognize pictures. The efficiency to recognize words
can be relatively lower than to recognize pictures as
indicated by the sensory–semantic theory (Nelson,
Reed, & McEvoy, 1977). The sensory–semantic theory
assumes that pictures hold two encoding advantages
over words: Pictures are perceptually more distinct
from one another than are words and are encoded
more uniquely to increase their chances for retrieval.
Pictures access meaning more directly than words.
Because conceptual processing makes a stronger contribution than perceptual processing to the advantage
of pictures over words in recognition (Stenberg, 2006),
people might be able to perform better with pictures
than with words on tasks in which relations among
stimuli were the focus of investigation.
One could deduce that deaf individuals would perform worse with words than with pictures for ordinary
concepts. This deduction seemed to be confirmed by
Ormel et al. (2010). Ormel et al. used written words
and colored line drawings as the stimuli and found
that the hearing children outperformed the deaf
children for words but relatively less so for pictures.
Although the hearing children’s performance was not
significantly different with pictures than with words,
the deaf children performed better with pictures than
with words. Obviously, a more complete image of deaf
individuals’ awareness of thematic and taxonomic relations might be revealed if pictures for ordinary entities
were used as well as words rather than if only words
were used as stimuli. Therefore, a task of forcedchoice decision making and a priming task of living–
nonliving categorization were conducted to test the
following hypothesis on Korean deaf adolescents in
the present study:
If Korean deaf adolescents had developed a different
pattern of awareness of taxonomic and thematic associations in comparison with hearing adolescents, then
they would be different from hearing adolescents in
reaction times and accuracies when making decisions
on taxonomic and thematic associations among ordinary
entities represented by pictures and written words.
Experiment 1
In a task of forced-choice decision making, participants have to compare the two relations among three
simultaneously presented items A, B, and C: A is
thematically associated with B and taxonomically related to C in a trial, and a decision has to be made as to
whether B goes better with A than C or C goes better
with A than B. Lin and Murphy (2001) came to a conclusion with this task that hearing adults have similar
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
appears less precise or less finely differentiated than
that of hearing children.
Children’s attention to taxonomic relations changes
with development (Markman & Hutchinson, 1984).
Some of the taxonomic knowledge of hearing preschoolers may remain to be developed (for review, see
Lucariello, Kyratzis, & Nelson, 1992), but 10-yearold hearing children can theoretically have welldeveloped conceptual organizations (Desprels-Fraysse
& Lecacheur, 1996). Young deaf children can be similar
to young hearing children in their conceptual organizations, but deaf adolescents and adults may perform
differently from hearing adolescents and adults in conceptual activities, respectively. It might be possible,
therefore, that deaf adolescents were aware of thematic
and taxonomic relations in a different way than hearing
adolescents.
Korean Deaf Adolescents’ Awareness of Thematic and Taxonomic Relations 379
strengths of thematic and taxonomic relations in their
conceptual organizations.
Methods
Participants. The students and their teachers from
two schools for the deaf in Korea responded to
a questionnaire, and eight deaf adolescents (five males,
M 5 13.0 years, age range: 12.0–14.0 years) were
selected as participants. Five of them had a hearing
loss of over 90 dB and three over 70 dB on the better
ear. Two of the participants received a poor level of
language intervention before going to elementary
schools, but the others had received a good level of
language intervention. One of them had a cochlear
implant, and five of them often had their hearing aids
on. One deaf adolescent had one deaf parent and one
had two deaf parents. According to their teachers’
responses, six of them were able to produce understandable oral expressions in everyday communications and two of them had achieved a reading
ability that paralleled that of hearing students of the
same age. They also received a modified version of
Raven Standard Progressive Matrices Test (RSPMT;
Zhang & Wang, 1985) and had a direct score of 74.5%.
Materials. As the result of a series of evaluations, 24
triads of words were selected, with 2 words (A and C)
for 2 exemplars of high typicality from a taxonomic
category and 1 word (B) thematically associated with A
in a triad, to make 24 thematic–taxonomic sets (see
Appendix A). Twenty-four triads of words were selected, with 2 words (A and B) thematically associated
and 1 word (C) not conceptually related to A in a triad,
to make 24 thematic sets. Twenty-four triads of words
were selected, with 2 words (A and C) for 2 exemplars
of high typicality from a taxonomic category and 1
word (B) not conceptually related to A in a triad, to
make 24 taxonomic sets. The familiarity of all the
items, the typicality of the taxonomically related items
(As and Cs of the taxonomic and thematic–taxonomic
sets), and the association strength between the thematically associated items (As and Bs of the thematic and
thematic–taxonomic sets) each were evaluated on a 7point scale by 25 college students (see Table 1).
Half of the thematic, taxonomic, and thematic–
taxonomic sets would be presented as pictures, respectively. The other half of the thematic, taxonomic, and
thematic–taxonomic sets would be presented as words,
respectively. Because it was inevitable that an item in
some sets was difficult to depict, a rule was accepted
according to which a set would be presented as words
if at least one of the three items was not easy to depict.
For example, ‘‘TV’’ and ‘‘elephant’’ are easy but
‘‘the spring festival’’ and ‘‘trip’’ are difficult to depict.
The pictures were not significantly different from the
words in typicality for the taxonomically related exemplars, t(94) 5.644, p 5 .521, in association strength for
the thematically associated exemplars, t(46) 5 .348,
p 5 .731, or in familiarity, t(166) 5 .631, p 5 .512,
for all the items.
An evaluation test of picture–word consistency
was done on a 7-point scale by another 25 college
students. The students each were provided with a list
of words on a piece of paper. Numbers one to seven
were marked on the right-hand side of each word. For
each picture shown on the screen in front of them with
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
The design formed a 2 3 2 3 3 (Hearing status [hearing, deaf] 3 Presentation mode [picture, word] 3
Category set [thematic, taxonomic, and thematic–
taxonomic]) complex factorial with hearing status as
between-participants variables and presentation mode
and category set within-participants variables. The
dependent variables were reaction times and accuracies.
The three levels of category set were thematic (A was
thematically associated with B but had no conceptual
relation with C), taxonomic (A was taxonomically related
to C but had no conceptual relation with B), and thematic–taxonomic (A was thematically associated with B
and taxonomically related to C) set. For a thematic or
taxonomic set, a strong conceptual relation would be
compared with a relation of null strength (A was not
conceptually related to B for a taxonomic set, or A was
not conceptually related to C for a thematic set). Deaf
adolescents would have a poorer performance than hearing adolescents for the thematic and taxonomic sets if
they were aware of conceptual relations less in general.
Similarly, 11 hearing adolescents (six males, M 5 13.0
years, age range: 12.0–14.0 years) with a direct
RSPMT score of 74.8% were recruited at random
from an elementary school.
380
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16:3 Summer 2011
Table 1 Characteristics of items of the thematic (Experiment 1 and Experiment 2), the taxonomic (Experiment 1 and
Experiment 2), and the thematic–taxonomic (Experiment 1) sets
Experiment 1
Item
Word lengtha (M 6 SD)
Thematic
A
B
C
A
B
C
A
B
C
Prime
Target
Prime
Target
1.96
1.95
2.03
2.33
2.25
2.25
2.07
2.02
2.25
1.98
1.94
2.24
2.26
Taxonomic
Thematic–taxonomic
Experiment 2
Thematic
Taxonomic
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
0.59
0.65
0.62
0.49
0.45
0.63
0.39
0.49
0.87
0.44
0.50
0.62
0.68
Familiarity (M 6 SD)
5.26
5.25
5.52
5.28
5.34
5.22
5.33
5.38
5.33
5.33
5.52
5.50
5.43
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
6
1.09
1.14
1.04
1.20
1.16
1.14
1.19
1.18
1.16
1.28
1.21
1.13
1.15
TP/ASb (M 6 SD)
6.30 6 0.24b
—
6.44 6 0.48
—
6.26 6 0.73
6.17 6 0.80
6.27 6 0.19**
6.28 6 0.93
6.32 6 0.27**
6.36 6 0.65
6.27 6 0.59
a
The Korean alphabet, Hangul, maps letters onto phonemes, and Hangul letters are printed in syllable blocks (SBs). The overall shape of a Korean word
appears similar to a Chinese word. For example, the word for cola is
. Most Korean words for ordinary concepts are composed of two or three SBs,
but many words that are of the highest familiarity are of one SB. Word length refers to the number of SB and was only calculated for the stimuli
presented as written words.
b
TP/AS means typicality or association strength. The values in this column marked with ‘‘**’’ were evaluation scores of association strength between As
and Bs in Experiment 1 or between primes and targets in Experiment 2. The values not marked with ‘‘**’’ were evaluation scores of typicality.
an overhead projector in a classroom, they must tick
one number beside the word to indicate how much it
represented the corresponding thing. The bigger the
number was the more the picture represented the
thing. The average score of evaluation of picture–word
consistency was 6.36 6 0.24 (M 6 SD). All the
pictures were made of 240 3 240 pixels. For example,
Figure 1 displays two triads of pictures (monkey,
banana, and elephant for one triad, and recorder, tape,
and radio for another).
Procedure. SuperLab 4.0 was used to control the experiment on personal computers (HP Compaq
nx6330). The screen resolution was 1280 3 800 pixels.
For each trial, a red fixation cross ‘‘1’’ remained at the
white screen centre for 800 ms. Then three items, A,
B, and C, were presented simultaneously to make a triangular contour with A the apex and B and C the two
base angles, respectively (see one triad of pictures in
Figure 1). Words were presented in Song-36. The
distance between B and C was 360 pixels, and A was
280 pixels vertically above B. Whether B was the base
angle on the left- or right-hand side was determined
randomly. The three items stayed on the screen for
3000 ms or until a key press was received. The intertrial interval was 1000 ms. Explanations were delivered to hearing adolescents in oral language and to
deaf adolescents in sign language. Participants were
Figure 1 Two triads of pictures in each of which participants had to decide whether the top picture was conceptually related
to the bottom left picture more than it was to the right one.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
Category set
Korean Deaf Adolescents’ Awareness of Thematic and Taxonomic Relations 381
Results
The data for the trials in which the participants’ reaction
times were below 200 ms or 3 SD above the average were
discarded. Totally 2.6% of the data were excluded.
Accuracies. For a thematic–taxonomic set, some participants decided that the taxonomic relation was
stronger than the thematic association but others
decided that the thematic association was stronger
than the taxonomic relation. The frequencies with
which the hearing and the deaf participants preferred
taxonomic to thematic associations when stimuli were
pictures and words were illustrated in Figure 2. A chi-
Figure 2 Frequencies of decision that 8 deaf and 11 hearing adolescents preferred taxonomic to thematic associations
for the 12 thematic–taxonomic sets presented as pictures and
words.
square test of the frequencies indicated that the hearing and the deaf participants seemed to have the same
tendency: They tended to prefer taxonomic to thematic associations among pictures more than among
words, v2 (1, N 5 243) 5 3.000, p 5 .083.
Data for the thematic and the taxonomic sets were
analyzed separately from those for the thematic–
taxonomic sets. A 2 3 2 3 2 (Hearing status 3 Presentation mode 3 Category set [thematic, taxonomic])
analysis of variance (ANOVA) of mixed measures to
the accuracy data showed that the main effect was significant for presentation mode, F1(1, 18) 5 19.51,
p 5 .000, g2 5 .534. The deaf and the hearing participants’ accuracies were significantly higher for pictures
(M 5 0.973, SE 5 0.012; M 5 0.977, SE 5 0.010)
than for words (M 5 0.849, SE 5 0.034; M 5 0.932,
SE 5 0.029), respectively. The other effects were not
significant.
Because the number of the deaf participants was
relatively small, a 2 3 2 3 2 (Hearing status 3 Presentation mode 3 Category set [thematic, taxonomic])
ANOVA of mixed measures to the accuracy data was
also done by item. Hearing status was the within-items
variables and presentation mode and category set
the between-items variables. Similar results were
obtained: The main effect was significant for presentation mode, F2(1, 44) 5 9.39, p 5 .004, g2 5 .176).
The other effects were not significant.
Reaction times. Reaction times were calculated for the
correct responses only. For the thematic–taxonomic sets,
all the responses were taken as correct. A 2 3 2 3 3
(Hearing status 3 Presentation mode 3 Category set)
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
also shown with demonstrations of how to make a decision by pressing the key ‘‘Z’’ or ‘‘/’’ (‘‘/’’ was horizontally separated from ‘‘Z’’ by eight keys).
The experiment was conducted in groups of three
to six deaf or hearing participants. Each participant
was led by an assistant (a graduate student in psychology) to one of the desks, which were separated from
one another by a distance of at least 2 m in a large
quiet room. Once seated, a participant was indicated
to turn on the laptop on his/her desk and get familiar
with the computer. Each of the assistants was in
charge of making one participant busy on his or her
computer before the experimenter (the second author)
came up to deliver the instruction.
There will be three words or three pictures on the
screen. The three items form a contour of triangle.
Please decide whether the top word/picture is conceptually related to the bottom left word/picture more
than it is to the right one. If so, press the key ‘‘Z’’
with the forefinger of the left hand. Otherwise, press
the key ‘‘/’’ with the forefinger of the right hand.
Explanations and demonstrations were shown to
one student after another. Only when the experimenter (a teacher from the school for deaf participants) thought that the student understood the
requirements and saw that he/she successively made
correct responses for 5 of the 12 practice trials could
the instruction delivery begin to the next student. The
practice trials were followed by 72 experimental trials.
The practice trials were randomized for each student
and so were the experimental trials.
382
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16:3 Summer 2011
203 ms) than with pictures (282 6 126 ms), t(70) 5
3.434, p 5 .001. The other effects were not significant.
Experiment 2
ANOVA of mixed measures to the reaction times showed
that the main effects were significant for presentation
mode, F1(1, 34) 5 32.70, p 5 .000, g2 5 .658,
and hearing status, F1(1, 34) 5 10.26, p 5 .005,
g2 5 .376. The participants’ reaction times were significantly longer when the stimuli were words (M 5 1325
ms, SE 5 59 ms) than when they were pictures
(M 5 1158 ms, SE 5 51 ms). The deaf participants’
reaction times (M 5 1411 ms, SE 5 81 ms) were significantly longer than those of the hearing participants
(M 5 1072 ms, SE 5 69 ms). The difference of reaction
times for the deaf participants was 237 6 115 ms when
the stimuli were changed from pictures into words,
whereas it was 97 6 133 ms for the hearing participants.
The other effects were not significant.
Similar results were obtained with the analysis by
item. The main effects were significant for presentation mode, F2(1, 66) 5 14.41, p 5 .000, g2 5 .179,
and for hearing status, F2(1, 66) 5 315.83, p 5 .000,
g2 5 .827. The two-way interaction between presentation mode and hearing status (see Figure 3)
was significant, F2(1, 66) 5 12.05, p 5 .001,
g2 5 .154. Simple analysis indicated that both the
deaf, t(70) 5 4.766, p 5 .000, and the hearing,
t(70) 5 2.038, p 5 .046, participants had significantly
longer reaction times for words than for pictures, but
the deaf participants’ reaction times were significantly
longer than those of the hearing ones for picturepresented stimuli, t(35) 5 13.434, p 5 .000, and
word-presented stimuli, t(35) 5 12.361, p 5 .000, respectively. Furthermore, the differences between the
deaf and the hearing participants in their reaction
times were significantly bigger with words (418 6
Methods
The design was the same as that of Experiment 1 except
that category set had two levels: thematic (primes were
thematically associated with targets) and taxonomic
(primes were taxonomically related to targets) set. No
thematic–taxonomic sets were used in Experiment 2.
Participants. Nine deaf adolescents (two males,
M 5 15.0 years, age range: 14.0–16.0 years) were selected in the same way as in Experiment 1, who had
a direct RSPMT score of 74.2%. Four of them had
a hearing loss of over 90 dB and five over 70 dB on
the better ear. Five of the participants received a poor
level of language intervention before going to elementary schools, but the others had received language intervention of a good level. One of them had a cochlear
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
Figure 3 The interactive effect of hearing status and presentation mode on participants’ reaction times.
In a task of forced-choice decision making, one may
have to form two relatively abstract representations for
the two relations among the three concrete items in
a trial and then conduct a logical calculation. Such
a comparison can be more dependent on language
and more time consuming than a direct comparison
of concrete entities. It did take a relatively long time
for the deaf (1411 6 311 ms) and for the hearing (1072
6 144 ms) participants to make a comparison of relations in a trial in Experiment 1. A priming task of
living–nonliving categorization is often used to examine individuals’ semantic organizations, in which decisions are made directly on concrete entities and
shorter reaction times are expected. In a trial in
Experiment 2, two thematically or taxonomically related stimuli would be sequentially presented, and participants would have to decide whether the second
stimulus represented a living or a nonliving thing. If
Korean deaf adolescents were aware of thematic and
taxonomic relations differently than hearing adolescents, then they would have different patterns of performance for the targets primed by thematically or
taxonomically related items in comparison with hearing adolescents in such a task.
Korean Deaf Adolescents’ Awareness of Thematic and Taxonomic Relations 383
Materials. The critical sets were 32 pairs of words and
32 pairs of pictures (see Appendix B): Sixteen pairs of
words were selected, with each pair of words for 2 exemplars (e.g., ‘‘heart’’ and ‘‘liver’’) of high typicality from
a taxonomic category (e.g., ‘‘body organs’’), to make 16
taxonomic word sets. Sixteen pairs of pictures were selected from the Internet, with each pair of pictures for 2
exemplars (e.g., ‘‘apple’’ and ‘‘grape’’) of high typicality
from a taxonomic category (e.g., ‘‘fruits’’), to make 16
taxonomic picture sets. Sixteen pairs of words were selected, with each pair of words for 2 ordinary things (e.g.,
‘‘onion’’ and ‘‘tear’’) that were thematically associated, to
make 16 thematic word sets. Sixteen pairs of pictures
were selected from the Internet, with each pair of pictures (e.g., ‘‘monkey’’ and ‘‘banana’’) for 2 ordinary
things that were thematically associated, to make 16 thematic picture sets. Materials were evaluated in the same
way as in Experiment 1 (see Table 1). The evaluations
score for picture–word consistency for the stimuli to be
presented as pictures was 6.29 6 0.33. The pictures were
not significantly different from the words in typicality for
the taxonomic sets, t(62) 5 .228, p 5 .820, in association
strength for the thematic sets, t(30) 5 .539, p 5 .594, or
in familiarity for all the items, t(126) 5 .635, p 5 .552.
Thirty-two pairs of words were selected for ordinary things to create 32 filler word sets. Thirty-two
pairs of pictures were selected for ordinary things to
create for 32 filler picture sets. The two members of
each filler set were not conceptually related with one
another and not conceptually related to those of any
critical sets. For half the critical and half the filler sets
and for half the pictures and half the words, the targets
were living entities. The pictures were of size of 240 3
240 pixels.
Procedure. The procedure was the same as that of
Experiment 1 except for the following. For each trial,
a red fixation cross was presented for 800 ms 240
pixels to the left- or right-hand side from the white
screen center. Then the prime was presented for 250
ms at the place where the fixation cross disappeared.
The screen was blank for 50 ms before the target was
shown 240 pixels to the left- or right-hand side of the
screen center. The target stayed on the screen for 3000
ms or until a response was received. The primes and
the targets were presented at the same side of the
screen for half the critical and half the filler sets and
for half the pictures and half the words. Participants
pressed the key ‘‘Z’’ if the target was a living and ‘‘/’’ if
it was a nonliving thing.
Results
The data for the trials in which the participants’ reaction times were below 200 ms or 3 SD above the
average were discarded. Totally 1.7% of the data were
deleted. A preliminary analysis indicated that the participants tended to have shorter reaction times or/and
higher accuracies when the prime or the target words
were of one syllable block (SB, e.g., -grass) than
-goat) or three
when they were of two (e.g.,
-lipstick) SBs in a trial, probably because
(e.g.,
one-SB words are easier to recognize than two- or
three-SB words. Data for another 11.0% trials were
excluded, in which the prime or the target words were
of one SB. Only data for the trials where stimuli were
presented on the same side of the screen would be
analyzed.
Accuracies. A 2 3 2 3 2 (Hearing status 3 Presentation mode 3 Category set) ANOVA of mixed measures to the accuracy data showed that the main
effects were significant for presentation mode, F1(1,
18) 5 35.85, p 5 .000, g2 5 .666, and for hearing
status, F1(1, 18) 5 52.58, p 5 .000, g2 5 .745. The
two-way interactions were significant between presentation mode and hearing status (see Figure 4), F1(1,
18) 5 13.58, p 5 .002, g2 5 .430, and between presentation mode and category set (see Figure 5), F1(1,
18) 5 55.60, p 5 .000, g2 5 .755. Analysis of simple
effect showed that the hearing participants’ accuracies
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
implant, and seven of them often had their hearing aids
on. One deaf adolescent had one deaf parent and two
had two deaf parents. According to their teachers’
responses, eight of them were able to produce understandable oral expressions in everyday communications
and five of them had achieved a reading ability that
paralleled that of hearing students of the same age.
Eleven hearing male adolescents (M 5 15.0 years, age
range: 14.0–16.0 years) with a direct RSPMT score
of 82.5% were recruited at random from a middle
school.
384
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16:3 Summer 2011
Similar results were obtained with the analysis
by item: The main effect was significant for hearing
status, F2(1, 27) 5 31.32, p 5 .000, g2 5 .566. The
two-way interaction between hearing status and presentation mode was significant, F2(1, 27) 5 10.05, p 5 .004,
g2 5 .295. The other effects were not significant.
Figure 5 The interactive effect of category set and presentation mode on participants’ accuracies.
Figure 6 The interactive effect of hearing status and category set on participants’ reaction times.
Figure 4 The interactive effect of hearing status and presentation mode on participants’ accuracies.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
(0.964 6 0.042, 0.903 6 0.070) were significantly
higher than those of the deaf participants (0.891 6
0.093, 0.635 6 0.109) when stimuli were pictures,
t(18) 5 2.400, p 5 .027, and words, t(18) 5 6.746,
p 5 .000, respectively. Both the hearing,
t(10) 5 3.023, p 5 .012, and the deaf, t(8) 5 4.413,
p 5 .003, participants had significantly lower accuracies for words than for pictures, but the deaf participants’ difference was significantly bigger than that of
the hearing ones, t(18) 5 3.684, p 5 .002. When the
stimuli were pictures, the participants’ accuracies were
significantly higher for the thematic (0.969 6 0.080)
than for the taxonomic (0.900 6 0.104) sets, t(19) 5
2.773, p 5 .012, but when the stimuli were written
words, they were significantly lower for the thematic
(0.717 6 0.188) than for the taxonomic (0.875 6
0.161) sets, t(19) 5 –4.790, p 5 .000. Although the
participants’ accuracies were not significantly different
between the word- and the picture-presented taxonomic sets, t(19) 5 .763, p 5 .455, they were significantly higher when stimuli were pictures than when
they were words for the thematic sets, t(19) 5 6.313,
p 5 .000. The other effects were not significant.
Reaction times. A similar analysis to the reaction time
data indicated that the main effect was significant for
category set, F1(1, 18) 5 36.91, p 5 .000, g2 5 .672.
The participants’ reaction times were significantly longer for the thematic (M 5 889 ms, SE 5 27 ms) than
for the taxonomic (M 5 757 ms, SE 5 35 ms) sets. The
main effects for hearing status, F1(1, 18) 5 5.29, p 5
.034, g2 5 .227, and presentation mode, F1(1, 18) 5
6.52, p 5 .020, g2 5 .266, and the two-way interaction
between hearing status and category set (see Figure 6),
F1(1, 18) 5 5.28, p 5 .034, g2 5 .227, were significant
at the significance level of .05. The difference of reaction times for the deaf participants (182 6 102 ms)
when taxonomic sets were changed into thematic sets
was significantly bigger than for the hearing participants
(82 6 91 ms), t(18) 5 2.243, p 5 .034. The other effects
were not significant.
Similar results were obtained with the analysis by
item: The main effects were significant for hearings status, F2(1, 27) 5 34.68, p 5 .000, g2 5 .591, and category sets, F2(1, 27) 5 8.27, p 5 .007, g2 5 .256. The
deaf participants, M 5 888 ms, SE 5 27 ms, had significantly longer reaction times than the hearing ones,
M 5 756 ms, SE 5 24 ms. The two-way interaction
between hearing status and category set was significant at
the significance level of .05, F2(1, 27) 5 4.76, p 5 .039,
g2 5 .166. The other effects were not significant.
Korean Deaf Adolescents’ Awareness of Thematic and Taxonomic Relations 385
General Discussion
Summary of Results
Thematic and Taxonomic Relations
Similarities. When comparing which of the two relations among three simultaneously presented items (in
Experiment 1) was relatively stronger than the other
one, the deaf participants’ pattern of awareness of thematic and taxonomic relations was not different from
that of the hearing participants. When deciding
whether the target was a living or nonliving thing
primed by a thematically or a taxonomically related
item (in Experiment 2), the deaf participants, again,
had the same pattern of performance as the hearing
participants in their accuracies. They were also not
different from the hearing participants in their reaction latencies for the taxonomically related items. All
these phenomena are in agreement with the findings
that deaf and hearing people have the same mode of
concept organizations for the things that can be perceived easily through vision (Osberger & Hesketh,
1986) and that deaf individuals’ conceptual organizations are generally similar to those of hearing
individuals (Marschark et al., 2004).
Differences. No matter whether the ANOVAs on participants’ data of reaction times and accuracies was
done by subject or by item, the results consistently
indicated that the deaf participants had poorer performance than the hearing ones. The deaf participants
needed more time than the hearing participants to
compare the two relations that coexisted among three
simultaneously presented items (Experiment 1). The
deaf participants’ responses to the targets, which were
taxonomically or thematically related to the primes,
appeared to be primed by the relations to a lesser
extent than those of the hearing participants (Experiment 2). These results revealed the deaf participants’
relatively weaker awareness of conceptual relations
than that of the hearing participants, consistent with
the previous findings that deaf children’s increase
in semantic categorization was limited across elementary school grade levels in comparison with hearing
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
Because deaf adolescents are likely to perform differently from hearing adolescents in conceptual activities
and tend to be relatively weaker at written language
than hearing adolescents in general, it was hypothesized that Korean deaf adolescents might be different
from hearing adolescents in their performance on thematically and taxonomically related entities presented
as words and pictures. A task of forced-choice decision
making (Experiment 1) and a priming task of living–
nonliving categorization (Experiment 2) were conducted with concrete concepts as stimuli represented
by pictures and written words. Consistent results were
obtained as the result of ANOVAs by subject and by
item. First, the deaf participants had longer reaction
times (in Experiment 1 and 2) and lower accuracies
(in Experiment 2) than the hearing ones. Second,
both the deaf and the hearing participants had longer
reaction times (in Experiment 1) and lower accuracies (in Experiment 1 and 2) for words than for
pictures, but the deaf participants had their accuracies lowered to a greater degree than the hearing
ones when stimuli were changed from pictures to
words.
Third, the deaf participants showed the same pattern of preference of conceptual relations as the hearing participants in Experiment 1. A clearer image,
however, seemed to be revealed of the influence of
category set on the participants’ performance in Experiment 2 than in Experiment 1: The accuracies of
both the deaf and the hearing participants were higher
for the thematic than for the taxonomic sets when the
stimuli were pictures, but they were lower for the
thematic than for the taxonomic sets when the stimuli
were words. Both the deaf and the hearing participants
had shorter reaction times with the taxonomic than
with the thematic sets, but the deaf participants had
bigger differences of reaction times than the hearing
participants when taxonomic sets were changed into
thematic sets. No matter whether the stimuli were
words or pictures, the deaf participants had their reaction times lengthened to a greater extent than the
hearing participants if taxonomic sets were changed
into thematic sets. They seemed to have a different
pattern of awareness of thematic and taxonomic relations than the hearing participants in the priming task
of living–nonliving categorization.
386
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16:3 Summer 2011
stimuli were words or pictures, seemed to indicate that
the deaf participants were more likely than the hearing
participants to prefer taxonomic to thematic associations. This was probably because of the combined
results of their relatively limited life experiences and
their relatively deeper dependence on school education
as summarized below.
Deaf adolescents in Korea tend to stay on campus
during school days. When they leave school, however,
their participation in social activities can be seriously
restricted because of language barrier, in comparison
with hearing adolescents. Because they mostly stay
home alone, their life experiences may not be as rich
as that of hearing adolescents. They can have fewer
opportunities than hearing adolescents to deal with
thematically related categories. On the contrary, they
do not have less time of school life than hearing adolescents, which may make them relatively more dependent on school activities to enrich their conceptual
knowledge. Thus Korean deaf adolescents’ awareness
may even stronger for taxonomic than for thematic
relations in comparison with their counterparts at least
if the stimuli were written words (Experiment 2),
although they can have a weaker awareness of
conceptual relations in general than hearing adolescents no matter whether the stimuli were words or
pictures.
Pictures and Words
Like the hearing participants, the deaf participants
had better performance with pictures than with words,
in line with the sensory–semantic theory (Nelson,
Reed, & McEvoy, 1977). Also similar to Ormel
et al., the present study further indicated that the deaf
participants found it even more difficult to make decisions with words than with pictures in comparison
with the hearing participants. There could be two
reasons for this phenomenon. First, deaf individuals
can be relatively more efficient than hearing individuals with pictures than with words in conceptual tasks
because of their relatively higher dependence on visual
perception than hearing individuals. However, this argument would need to be supported with further
studies. Second, deaf Korean adolescents can be
generally poorer than hearing adolescents at written
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
children (Ormel et al., 2010), that deaf adolescents
were aware of the relations between items less than
hearing adolescents (Li & Zhang, 2009), and that deaf
adults had weaker awareness of the relations between
entities in general than hearing adults (Marschark
et al., 2004).
What seemed more interesting was the difference
between the deaf and the hearing participants in their
relative awareness of taxonomic and thematic associations. The processing of thematic associations can be less
resource-demanding than that of taxonomic relations.
For example, neuroimaging studies indicated that the
processing of thematic relations might demand less cognitive resource than that of taxonomic relations (Sachs
et al., 2008). Furthermore, uneducated people (Scribner,
1974), young children (Scheuner et al., 2004), and elder
people (Pennequin et al., 2006) have a tendency to prefer
thematic to taxonomic relations in cognitive activity.
Thus, both deaf and hearing individuals might not have
a poorer performance with thematic associations than
with taxonomic relations. Actually, the deaf and the hearing participants both got higher accuracies for the thematic than for the taxonomic sets when the stimuli were
pictures in Experiment 2.
However, middle school students or elementary
school students of high grades, who have received
years of education, can have to deal with thematic
categories relatively less frequently than with taxonomic categories. Taxonomic relations are very important for them to learn science knowledge (Vygotsky,
1999). Consequently, teenager students may have developed an unconscious bias to make use of taxonomic
more often than of thematic associations in cognitive
activities. Thus, the results of Experiment 2 did indicate that both the deaf and the hearing participants
needed less time to be aware of the taxonomic than of
the thematic relations among the word- and picturepresented entities and that both the deaf and the hearing participants were more accurate to be aware of the
taxonomic than of the thematic relations among the
word-presented entities that people experience often
in everyday life.
However, the result of the deaf participants’ even
bigger difference in their reaction times for the thematic and the taxonomic sets than that of the hearing
participants (Experiment 2), no matter whether the
Korean Deaf Adolescents’ Awareness of Thematic and Taxonomic Relations 387
Korean. In addition, deaf adolescents might learn
Korean as a second language. To support this claim,
it was shown that that college (Li, Zhang, & Wang,
2010) and high school (Li, Li, & Yuan, 2010) Chinese
students performed worse in conceptual activities with
English words than with pictures or Chinese words.
Similarly, the Korean deaf adolescents’ performance
could have been poorer for words than for
pictures, simply because of their low proficiency of
Korean.
Korea has a population of about forty million. The
number of students in schools for the deaf is relatively
limited. Thus, relatively small samples of participants
were recruited in the present study. In addition, the
participants of Experiment 1 (12.0–14.0 years) were
younger than those of Experiment 2 (14.0–16.0 years).
Although 10-year-old hearing children can theoretically have well-developed conceptual organizations
(Desprels-Fraysse & Lecacheur, 1996), no conclusion
has been reached on the deaf in general. The difference in the deaf participants’ age might have contributed to the difference in their performance between
the two experiments. Because comparisons were made
between the deaf and the hearing participants of the
same age within each of the experiments, the similarities and differences between the deaf and the hearing
adolescents seemed to exist as to how they were aware
of thematic and taxonomic relations with pictures and
words in forced-choice decision making and living–
nonliving categorization.
Conclusions
In conclusion, Korean deaf adolescents’ awareness of
thematic and taxonomic relations among concepts for
ordinary entities was relatively weaker than those of
hearing adolescents. Both deaf and hearing adolescents
tended to be more readily aware of taxonomic than of
thematic associations and had a better performance
with pictures than with words, but deaf adolescents
had an even bigger advantage than hearing adolescents
for taxonomic over thematic associations and for pictures over words. Although there might be some
limitations to the generalization of these findings be-
Funding
Korea Foundation for Academic Scholars International Scholar Exchange Fellowship program, 2009–
2010.
Conflict of Interest
No conflicts of interest were reported.
Acknowledgements
The authors wanted to show their thanks to the lovely
adolescents from Daegu Younghwa School for the
Deaf, Sungsim School for the Deaf, Yeungnam Middle School, and Yeounil Elementary School, Republic
of Korea, for their participation in the experiments as
participants.
References
Auer, E. T. Jr., & Bernstein, L. E. (2008). Estimating when and
how words are acquired: A natural experiment on the
development of the mental lexicon. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 750–775.
Blank, A., & Bridger, H. W. (1966). Conceptual cross-modal transfer in deaf and hearing children. Child Development, 37, 29–38.
Blaye, A., & Bonthoux, F. (2001). Thematic and taxonomic
relations in preschoolers: The development of flexibility
in categorization choices. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 19, 395–412.
Blewitt, P., & Toppino, T. C. (1991). The development of taxonomic structure in lexical memory. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 51, 196–310.
Borghi, A. M., & Caramelli, N. (2003). Situation bounded conceptual organization in children: From action to spatial
relations. Cognitive Development, 18, 49–60.
Courtin, C. (1997). Does sign language provide deaf children with
an abstraction advantage? Evidence from a categorization task.
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2, 162–171.
Desprels-Fraysse, A., & Lecacheur, M. (1996). Children’s conception of object, as revealed by their categorizations. The
Journal of Genetic Psychology, 157, 49–64.
Gastgeb, H. Z., Strauss, M. S., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). Do
individuals with autism process categories differently? The
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
Limitations
cause of the limited numbers of participants across
experiments, the results can nevertheless be helpful
to remind educators for the deaf that more effort
should be made to explicitly teach deaf students the
structure of knowledge so as to strengthen their awareness of taxonomic and in particular thematic relations.
388
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16:3 Summer 2011
Monreal, S. T., & Hernández, R. S. (2005). Reading levels of
Spanish deaf students. American Annals of the Deaf, 15,
379–387.
Nelson, D. L., Reed, V. S., & McEvoy, C. L. (1977). Learning to
order pictures and words: A model of sensory and semantic
encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Learning and Memory, 3, 485–497.
Ormel, E. A., Gijsel, M. A. R., Hermans, D., Bosman, A. M.
T., Knoors, H., & Verhoeven, L. (2010). Semantic
categorization: A comparison between deaf and hearing
children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43,
347–360.
Osberger, M., & Hesketh, L. (1986). Speech and language
disorders related to hearing impairment. In N. Lass,
NorthernJ., L. McReynolds, & D. Yoder (Eds.), Handbook
of speech-language pathology and audiology (pp. 858–886).
Toronto, Canada: BC Decker.
Paul, P. V. (1987). Deaf children’s comprehension of multi-meaning
words: Research and implications. Symposium presentation,
the Annual Indiana Association for Child and Adults with
Learning Disabilities (13th), Indianapolis, IN, October.
Pennequin, V., Fontaine, R., Bonthoux, F., Scheuner, N., &
Blaye, A. (2006). Categorization deficit in old age: Reality
or artifact? Journal of Adult Development, 13, 1–9.
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., &
Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories.
Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.
Sachs, O., Weis, S., Zellagui, N., Huber, W., Zvyagintsev, M., &
Kircher, T. (2008). Automatic processing of semantic relations in fMRI: Neural activation during semantic priming
of taxonomic and thematic categories. Brain Research, 1218,
194–205.
Scheuner, N., Bonthoux, F., Cannard, C., & Blaye, A. (2004).
The role of associative strength and conceptual relations in
matching tasks in 4- and 6-year-old children. International
Journal of Psychology, 39, 290–304.
Scribner, S. (1974). Developmental aspects of categorized
recall in a West African society. Cognitive Psychology, 6,
475–494.
Stenberg, G. (2006). Conceptual and perceptual factors in the
picture superiority effect. European Journal of Cognitive
Psychology, 18, 813–847.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1999). Thinking and language. Hongzhou,
China: Zhejiang Education Press.
Zarfaty, Y., Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (2004). The performance
of young deaf children in spatial and temporal number
tasks. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 9,
315–326.
Zhang, H., & Wang, X. (1985). The Manual of Raven Standard
Progressive Matrices Test. Beijing, China: Beijing University
Press.
Zhang, J., Li, D., & Wu, X. (2008). Deaf adolescents’ representations of taxonomic relations. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 40,
1178–1189.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
effect of typicality and development. Child Development, 77,
1717–1729.
Gaustad, M. G., Kelly, R. R., Payne, J. A., & Lylak, E. (2002).
Deaf and hearing students’ morphological knowledge applied
to printed English. American Annals of the Deaf, 147, 6–21.
Goldin-Meadow, S., Gelman, S. A., & Mylander, C. (2005).
Expressing generic concepts with and without a language
model. Cognition, 96, 109–126.
Harris, M., & Moreno, C. (2004). Deaf children’s use of phonological coding: Evidence from reading, spelling, and
working memory. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 9, 253–268.
Jerger, S., & Damian, M. F. (2005). What’s in a name? Typicality and relatedness effects in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 46–75.
Kyle, F. E., & Harris, S. (2006). Concurrent correlates and
predictors of reading and spelling achievement in deaf
and hearing school children. Journal of Deaf Studies and
Deaf Education, 11, 273–288.
Lederberg, A. R., & Spencer, P. E. (2009). Word-learning abilities in deaf and hard-of-hearing preschoolers: Effect of
lexicon size and language modality. Journal of Deaf Studies
and Deaf Education, 14, 44–62.
Li, D., Li, J., & Yuan, D. (2010). Non-English majors’ awareness
of thematic and taxonomic relations. Foreign Language
Teaching and Research, 42, 131–137.
Li, D., & Zhang, J. (2009). Chinese deaf adolescents’ free recall
of taxonomic, thematic, and slot-filler categories. Journal of
Scandinavian Psychology, 50, 355–366.
Li, D., Zhang, X., & Wang, G. (2010). Senior Chinese high
school students’ awareness of thematic and taxonomic
relations in L1 and L2. Bilingualism: Language and
Cognition. Advance online publication. doi:10.1017/
S1366728910000416.
Lin, E. L., & Murphy, G. L. (2001). Thematic relations in
adults’ concepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 3–28.
Lucariello, J., Kyratzis, A., & Nelson, K. (1992). Taxonomic knowledge: What kind and when? Child Development, 63, 978–998.
Lucariello, J., & Nelson, K. (1985). Slot-filler categories as
memory organizers for young children. Developmental Psychology, 21, 272–282.
Mandler, J. M. (1977). Categorical and schematic organization
in memory. In C. R. Puff (Ed.), Memory organization and
structure (pp. 259–299). New York: Academic Press.
Markman, E. M., & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children’s sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus
thematic relations. Cognitive Psychology, 16, l–27.
Marschark, M., Convertino, C., McEvoy, C., & Masteller, A.
(2004). Organization and use of the mental lexicon by deaf
and hearing individuals. Cognition, 149, 51–61.
Marschark, M., Lang, H. G., & Albertini, J. A. (2002). Educating the deaf students: From Research to Practice. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Korean Deaf Adolescents’ Awareness of Thematic and Taxonomic Relations 389
Appendix A
Thematic, taxonomic, and thematic–taxonomic sets presented in the form of pictures and written words used as stimuli in
Experiment 1
Presentation mode
A
B
C
Taxonomic
Word
wardrobe
apple juice
blue
wolf
teacher
pillow
tank
parrot
spoon
earthquake
traditional hog
copper
bed
coca cola
green
tiger
doctor
beddings
missile
dove
dish
typhoon
apartment
iron
coal
towel
rice-cooker
birthday
mid-autumn day
school
toilet
library
fog
pepper
national road
fish
Picture
TV
pencil
apple
walnut
necklace
crocodile
lion
passenger
rice plant
T-shirt
football
cucumber
air conditioner
pen
grape
pine nuts
bracelet
lizard
bear
fishing boat
barley
skirt
basketball
cabbage
praying mantis
peanut
bicycle
jeans
corn
dagger
chair
tongue
cat
pine
waterfall
camera
Word
onion
window
cloud
ribbon
lipstick
pumpkin
Needle
Button
Soldier
temple
big spoon
cigarette
tear
glass
sky
present
lips
pumpkin soup
string
clothes
gun
monks
cooked rice
lung cancer
park
ball pen
red
pizza
earth
bottle
river
wheat
bread
sweet potato
brain
soap
Picture
egg
wallet
hammer
bee
light house
lock
blackboard
wheel
balloon
match
stamp
toothbrush
chicken
money
nail
beehive
sea
key
chalk
car
children
fire
letter
teeth
oil boat
lettuce
orchid
strawberry
tomato
an eagle
whale
computer
sofa
railway
knife
chopsticks
Thematic
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
Category set
390
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 16:3 Summer 2011
Appendix
Continued
Presentation mode
A
B
C
Thematic–taxonomic
Word
umbrella
orange
train
pistol
ring
rose
goat
milk
magpie
pig
truck
refrigerator
rain
juice
trip
policeman
marriage
thorn
grass
cow
the spring festival
pork
load
ice
raincoat
peach
plane
rifle
earring
orchid
sheep
coffee
crow
dog
passenger-car
washing machine
Picture
telephone
feet
bus
camel
recorder
butterfly
bookshelf
spider
sparrow
eyes
skiing
monkey
ear
shoes
driver
desert
tape
flower
book
spider-web
electrical wire
glasses
snow
banana
cell phone
hands
taxi
giraffe
radio
dragonfly
dinner table
ant
swallow
nose
swimming
elephant
Appendix B
Thematically and taxonomically related items presented in the form of pictures and written words used as stimuli in
Experiment 2
Category set
Presentation mode
Prime
Target
Taxonomic
Picture
shark
grape
dragonfly
dove
lizard
herb
feet
barley
the moon
cell phone
trousers
pleasure boat
TV
necklace
spoon
old-styled house
whale
apple
ant
swallow
snake
orchid
hands
rice
the earth
telephone
T-shirt
fishing boat
refrigerator
earring
chopsticks
western-styled house
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
Category set
Korean Deaf Adolescents’ Awareness of Thematic and Taxonomic Relations 391
Appendix
Continued
Category set
Prime
Target
Word
mackerel
dog
bull
heart
nose
cucumber
lion
teacher
coca cola
sea
wardrobe
yellow
beddings
iron
birthday
flood
bus
egg
desert
balloon
carrot
monkey
butterfly
toothbrush
stamp
cake
lock
blackboard
sparrow
spider
wallet
eyes
hairtail fish
cat
sheep
liver
tongue
lettuce
tiger
doctor
apple juice
mountain
bed
red
pillow
copper
the mid-autumn day
typhoon
driver
chicken
camel
children
horse
banana
flower
teeth
letter
candle
key
chalk
electrical wire
spider web
money
glasses
goat
skirt
pistol
pork
nest
pumpkin soup
temple
lipstick
onion
umbrella
train
magpie
ring
taxi
cigarette
car
grass
woman
policeman
pig
bird
pumpkin
monks
lips
tear
rain
trip
the spring festival
marriage
charge
lung cancer
accident
Picture
Word
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article-abstract/16/3/375/431564 by guest on 15 June 2020
Thematic
Presentation mode
Descargar