Subido por tzespina

Habitat selection response of the freshwater shrimp Atyaephyra desmarestii

Anuncio
Science of the Total Environment 662 (2019) 816–823
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Science of the Total Environment
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/scitotenv
Habitat selection response of the freshwater shrimp Atyaephyra
desmarestii experimentally exposed to heterogeneous copper
contamination scenarios
Victoria C. Vera-Vera a, Francisco Guerrero a,b, Julián Blasco c, Cristiano V.M. Araújo c,⁎
a
b
c
Department of Animal Biology, Plant Biology and Ecology, University of Jaén, 23071 Jaén, Spain
Center of Advanced Studies in Earth Sciences, University of Jaén, 23071 Jaén, Spain
Department of Ecology and Coastal Management, Institute of Marine Sciences of Andalusia, CSIC, 11510 Puerto Real, Cádiz, Spain
H I G H L I G H T S
G R A P H I C A L
A B S T R A C T
• The ability of the freshwater shrimp
Atyaephyra desmarestii to avoid Cu was
tested.
• Gradients and patches of contamination
by copper were simulated in the
HeMHAS.
• In linear contamination gradient,
shrimps significantly avoided copper.
• In heterogeneous contamination scenarios, shrimps' distribution tended to
be random.
• In meta-ecosystem scenarios, clean
areas prevented organisms of a continuous exposure.
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 26 December 2018
Received in revised form 22 January 2019
Accepted 23 January 2019
Available online 24 January 2019
Editor: Damia Barcelo
Keywords:
Atyaephyra desmarestii
Avoidance
Habitat connectivity
HeMHAS
Non-forced exposure
Preference
a b s t r a c t
In contaminated aquatic ecosystems, it is expected that organisms suffer some effects caused by the contaminants. However, for mobile organisms inhabiting heterogeneously contaminated ecosystems, the continuous exposure to contaminants can be avoided by moving to less contaminated habitats. The present study evaluated the
habitat selection of the freshwater shrimp Atyaephyra desmarestii experimentally exposed to different copper
concentrations to verify whether the heterogeneous contamination distribution and the connectivity between
habitats with different copper levels could generate a random population distribution similar to metapopulation.
The experiments were performed in the HeMHAS (Heterogeneous Multi-Habitat Assay System), a non-forced
multi-compartmented exposure system, in which it is possible to simulate the distribution of contaminants in
a linear gradient or as patches of contamination. Copper was used to simulate a linear contamination gradient
(26 to 105 μg/L Cu) and two patchy scenarios with three contamination levels [reference zone (R: 26 ± 7 μg/L
Cu), mixing zone (M: 61 ± 2 μg/L Cu) and disturbed zone (D: 101 ± 12 μg/L Cu)], with two mixing zones or
one central mixing zone in a heterogeneous scenario. In the copper gradient scenario, a clear trend of shrimps
(59.6 ± 8.0% of the population) moving to the reference zones and an avoidance of 66.7 ± 11.1% of the most contaminated zone were observed. For the patchy scenarios, a random distribution of organisms (34, 36 and 30% for
R, M and D zones, respectively) was observed in the scenario with one mixing zone; on the other hand, a slight
preference for the reference zones (44.9 ± 4.8%) was evidenced in the scenario with two mixing zones. As
shrimps are able to select less contaminated areas, it is highly important to preserve clean zones in heterogeneously contaminated environments, such as the arrangement in meta-ecosystems, as the less- or
⁎ Corresponding author.
E-mail address: [email protected] (C.V.M. Araújo).
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2019.01.304
0048-9697/© 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
V.C. Vera-Vera et al. / Science of the Total Environment 662 (2019) 816–823
817
uncontaminated zones might represent less stressful areas to protect populations against continuous contamination exposure.
© 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Ecosystems are complex units that integrate multiple interactions
between the environment and organisms. When ecosystems are connected, those interactions drive their dynamics, homeostasis as well as
the exchanges between them (Fleeger et al., 2003; Ewers et al., 2007;
Fuller et al., 2015) such as the meta-ecosystem concept described by
Loreau et al. (2003). Due to this connectivity, the environmental contamination in an ecosystem could affect other undisturbed adjacent
ones (Spromberg et al., 1998; Clements and Rohr, 2009; Moe et al.,
2013). One of the possible consequences to occur in contaminationaffected ecosystems is the evasion of the organisms avoiding the most
disturbed areas (Ǻtland and Barlaup, 1995; Hansen et al., 1999b;
Tierney, 2016), which could generate a dynamic similar to the metapopulation and metacommunity arrangement (Grimm et al., 2003). When
organisms decide to escape from disturbed areas, the community structure and dynamics of the avoided areas, as well as the adjacent ones are
likely to be affected (Exley, 2000; Fleeger et al., 2003; Wells et al., 2004).
Although escaping is a solution for the survival of the avoiders (Moe
et al., 2013), the effects to ecosystems could lead to the loss of abundance and biodiversity (Spromberg et al., 1998; Lopes et al., 2004).
This change in the composition of the community might still exert influence on the ecological relationships such as competition by space and
food and predation pressure in the destination area (Hansen et al.,
1999a; Exley, 2000; Fleeger et al., 2003; Wells et al., 2004).
Traditionally, the potential impact of contaminants on organisms is
determined by ecotoxicity tests (Calow, 1989; Maltby and Calow,
1989) that provide important evidence of the contamination-caused
environmental disturbance (Faggio et al., 2016; Capillo et al., 2018), although the experimental conditions are a little reductionist when compared to the environmental complexity (Straalen, 2003; Segner et al.,
2014; Khan, 2018). For instance, the exposure of organisms to contaminants in ecotoxicity tests is normally performed in confined environments (forced exposure), in which organisms are mandatorily and
continuously in contact with the contaminants. Therefore, no option
to escape from the contaminants and to avoid their toxic effects is considered. However, some studies have applied non-forced exposure as an
alternative method to assess toxicity (Jones, 1947, 1948; Folmar, 1976;
Gunn and Noakes, 1986; Hartwell et al., 1989; Smith and Bailey, 1990;
Richardson et al., 2001). In the last decade, a non-forced, free-choice,
multi-compartmented exposure system developed by Lopes et al.
(2004) and slightly modified by Rosa et al. (2012) has proved to be a
suitable tool to assess contamination-driven spatial avoidance (Araújo
et al., 2016a; Vasconcelos et al., 2016; Silva et al., 2017, 2018). The aim
of the approach based on multi-compartmented exposure systems is
to analyze the displacement of organisms between multiple compartments with different levels of contamination and then to verify how
the contaminants drive the spatial distribution of organisms (Lopes
et al., 2004; Moreira-Santos et al., 2008; Rosa et al., 2012; Araújo et al.,
2016b, 2016c, 2018a, 2018b; Islam et al., 2019). Although this approach
does not solve the problems related to the shortcomings of the environmental relevance of ecotoxicity tests, it provides a complementary
method to simulate environmental heterogeneity regarding contamination. Evidence of the spatial avoidance response in multicompartmented systems has been observed in laboratory studies with
several aquatic organisms such as: copepods (Araújo et al., 2014a), cladocerans (Lopes et al., 2004), mollusks (Araújo et al., 2012), decapods
(Araújo et al., 2019), amphibians (Araújo et al., 2014b) and fish
(Moreira-Santos et al., 2008; Silva et al., 2017, 2018) exposed to different contamination sources.
The spatial avoidance response is a potential mechanism used by mobile organisms to prevent toxicity (Wells et al., 2004; Tierney et al., 2010;
Hellou, 2011), which could be expected in habitats where there is heterogeneity in the contamination distribution and if the connectivity between
ecosystems is considered (Kimball and Levin, 1985). Taking into account
that generally neither the toxicants nor organisms are uniformly distributed in space (Spromberg et al., 1998), a new non-forced, multicompartmented exposure system (HeMHAS - Heterogeneous MultiHabitat System Assay; Araújo et al., 2018b) has been developed recently
to simulate heterogeneously contaminated habitats. The HeMHAS
makes it possible to assess the dynamics of the displacement of organisms
between habitats and the processes of contamination avoidance in a more
complex scenario, as organisms can move freely among different zones
(Araújo et al., 2018b). When organisms are exposed in a non-forced,
multi-compartmented exposure system, the concept of environmental
disturbance/impact goes beyond the toxicity at the individual level and
its focus turns towards the spatial distribution and habitat preference, as
when organisms displace to less aversive environment no toxicity is necessarily expected to occur (De Lange et al., 2006; Tierney et al., 2010).
If we consider that the dispersion of contaminants can also affect the
populations' spatial distribution in adjacent undisturbed habitats
(Spromberg et al., 1998; Moe et al., 2013), assessing the organisms' habitat preference, and therefore their spatial distribution, could help to understand the dynamics of the displacement of organisms in wider
geographic areas (considering connected ecosystems), thus integrating
the communities and landscape ecology: the meta-ecosystem framework (Loreau et al., 2003). Although, the organisms' preferential distribution tends to be towards the less contaminated habitat in an
environment with a contamination gradient, we hypothesize that, in a
heterogeneous (patchy) contamination scenario, the organisms might
tend to present a dynamic of dispersion with a random (not preferential) displacement between habitats, where less/uncontaminated habitats might play a crucial role as refuges to prevent toxicity. To test this
hypothesis, the shrimp Atyaephyra desmarestii was exposed to copper
in the HeMHAS as previous studies have shown that this species is
able to detect and avoid contamination (Araújo et al., 2019) and copper
in particular (Araújo et al., 2018c). The aim of the present study was,
firstly, to assess the ability of this species to avoid copper and select a
less disturbed habitat when exposed to copper in a heterogeneous contamination scenario. Secondly, it was intended to assess whether the
presence of undisturbed habitats in a heterogeneous contamination
scenario allows organisms to display a very dynamic and random distribution, in which it is not possible to detect a clear avoidance of the most
contaminated environments.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Test organism
The freshwater shrimp A. desmarestii (Millet, 1831) was used as the
test organism. This species is distributed along Mediterranean and Central European regions (Van den Brink and Van Der Velde, 1986; Moog
et al., 1999; Tittizer et al., 2000; Fidalgo and Gerhardt, 2002). Shrimps
were caught by a hand net in an uncontaminated area (Araújo et al.,
2018c, 2019) from the river Guadalete (36°47′55.05″ N y 5°19′53.04″
W). After sampling, shrimps were transported to the laboratory where
they were acclimated for one week before the experiments. Acclimation
was performed in Guadalete river water during the first two days and
afterwards the river water was gradually replaced by old dechlorinated
tap water: 25% on day 3, 50% on day 4 and 100% on day 5. Shrimps were
818
V.C. Vera-Vera et al. / Science of the Total Environment 662 (2019) 816–823
maintained in 40 L aquaria, with continuous aeration, at 20 °C and photoperiod of 12/12 (light/dark). No food was provided during acclimation. Only shrimps with 2.5 to 2.6 cm were used in the experiments to
avoid differences in the response due to the life stage. Mortality was
lower than 5% during acclimation.
2.2. HeMHAS – heterogeneous multi-habitat assays system
The HeMHAS (70 × 30 × 7 cm: length, width and height; Araújo et al.,
2018b) is a non-forced, multi-compartmented exposure system formed
by 18, 6 cm high, circular compartments, 10 cm in diameter and a capacity
of 320 mL, which are distributed in a 3 × 6 arrangement (3 compartments
on a vertical axis in each one of 6 zones on a longitudinal axis) (Fig. 1). All
compartments are connected to all the adjacent compartments. The bottom of the compartments is concave and presents a smooth ascending
slope that reaches the connections. The opening and closing of the connections between the compartments is controlled by gates at the connections. A lid for each compartment prevents the organisms from jumping
out. The system was constructed in acetal (polyoxymethylene) to prevent
adsorption of contaminants. The HeMHAS was patented by the Spanish
Patent and Trademark Office (#P201731426).
A schematic representation of the zones and their distribution composing different contamination scenarios is shown in Fig. 2. For each
scenario, control experiments (no copper, only tap water in all the compartments) were performed to prove that the organisms' distribution is
random in the absence of contamination. In total, six experiments were
performed, which were identified by letters as indicated in Fig. 2. The
experiments were performed in triplicate and lasted 3 h.
In the experiments with copper and before introducing the copper
concentrations into the compartments, the gates were closed to isolate
the compartments from each other and to avoid the mixing of the concentrations. In the control experiments, all the compartments were
filled with the same tap water used in the acclimation of organisms. Afterwards, one shrimp was put in each compartment (load of one organism per 320 mL) and then the gates were placed with the hole
positioned down, allowing the organisms to move between compartments. The organisms' position was recorded at each 30 min for the control experiments (no copper) and at each 15 min for the experiments
with copper during the 3 h exposure period. Shorter observation periods were used in experiments with copper in order to detect a possible
increase in the displacement of organisms stimulated by copper.
2.4. Statistical analysis
2.3. Habitat selection experiments
Different experiments with different contamination scenarios were
simulated: experiments with a linear copper gradient and two groups
of experiments with copper contamination patches using different spatial distributions of copper and different configurations of connectivity
between compartments. In each scenario, three copper levels were
used: 0 μg/L (reference zone - R), 50 μg/L (mixing zone - M) and 150
μg/L (disturbed zone - D). This range was chosen because copper concentrations described to trigger avoidance in cladocerans and fish varies
between 15 and 180 μg/L (Moreira-Santos et al., 2008; Silva et al., 2018;
Islam et al., 2019; see also the review by Araújo and Blasco, 2019). Due
to the mix occurring in the HeMHAS, the concentrations in each zone
were determined at the end of the experiments by ICP-MS (Agilent
7900; with a detection limit of 0.0025 μg/L). Each zone is represented
by six compartments, which were distributed in groups of two to
three contiguous compartments. Zones with the same contamination
level were coded with a number, such as R1 and R2, indicating in this
case that there are two reference zones; the same was applied to the
other zones. The mean copper concentration values ranged as follows
(experiments E and F): 26 ± 7 (zone R), 61 ± 2 (zone M) and 101 ±
12 (zone D) μg/L. For experiment D with a copper gradient, the concentrations measured were: 26 ± 4 (R1), 33 ± 1 (R2), 49 ± 1 (M1), 71 ± 2
(M2), 92 ± 3 (D1) and 105 ± 9 μg/L (D2).
The distributions (in %) of the shrimps (after arcsine transformation)
in the control (using only dechlorinated tap water) and avoidance (with
the copper gradient) tests were evaluated by a mixed-design (repeated
measures) ANOVA, in which time was considered a repeated measure
(within-subjects factor) and compartments were treated as a
between-subjects factor. The sphericity of the repeated measures was
checked by using Mauchly's test to correct the degrees of freedom if
necessary; if sphericity was violated (the variances of the differences
are not equal: P b 0.05), the Greenhouse-Geisser correction for degrees
of freedom was applied (see Tables with Mauchly's test in Supplementary material). When statistically significant differences (P b 0.05) were
observed for any factor (time or compartment), the Bonferroni post hoc
test was employed to identify differences in the percentage of organisms for time and compartments. When the response did not vary
with time (results of the repeated measures ANOVA were not significant; P N 0.05), the data of the organisms' distribution were considered
as the means from the different observation times (0.5, 1.0, 1.5, 2.0, 2.5
and 3.0 h for the control experiments and 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1.0, 1.25, 1.5,
1.75, 2.0, 2.25, 2.5, 2.75 and 3.0 h for the experiments with copper).
For experiment D (with a copper gradient), the quantification of the
number of organisms that avoided (avoiders) copper was calculated per
zone (concentrations) as described by Moreira-Santos et al. (2008):
Avoiders ¼ NE −N O
Fig. 1. Diagram of the HeMHAS. The lid and the gate used, respectively, to cover the
compartments and to open or close the connections between compartments are
represented on the right. The displacement of the organisms is possible when the gate is
placed with the hole positioned down (Araújo et al., 2018b).
where NE is the number of organisms expected in a given zone and
NO represents the number of organisms observed in a given zone. For
the zones with the highest copper concentration, NE was the number
of organisms introduced at the beginning (6 shrimps: 1 for each compartment) of the experiment (it is supposed that no organism is expected to move from another zone towards this one). For the other
zones, NE was the number of organisms inserted at the beginning of
the test plus the organisms introduced in the adjacent zone with a
higher copper concentration (it is assumed that organisms move towards a less contaminated zone). As each zone contained six shrimps
(from six compartments), the NE values (from the most to the least contaminated zones:) were 6, 12 and 18 for the zones D, M and R, respectively. For a given zone, NO was determined considering the organisms
inhabiting the zone studied and the organisms present in the highest
copper concentration. For example, for the most contaminated zone,
NO was the number of organisms observed in that zone (considering
the six compartments, as there is no a most contaminated zone); for
V.C. Vera-Vera et al. / Science of the Total Environment 662 (2019) 816–823
819
Fig. 2. Levels of copper, the copper contamination scenarios and their respective control experiments used to assess the habitat selection response of Atyaephyra desmarestii. Experiments
A, B and C represent the control experiments (on the left), while the experiments with copper are indicated by the letters D, E and F (on the right). Experiment D represents a linear copper
contamination gradient and experiments E and F represent heterogeneous copper contamination patches, differing regarding the connections between compartments and the disposition
of the mixing zones.
the adjacent zone (M), NO represents the organisms observed in that
zone plus the organisms present in the most contaminated zone; for
the less contaminated zone, NO is defined by the organisms observed
in that zone and in the zones with higher copper levels (i.e., sum of all
the organisms in the system). The following equation was used to calculate the percentage of avoidance for each zone (concentration):
Avoidance ð%Þ ¼
Avoiders
100:
NE
The AC50 (concentration that triggers an avoidance of 50% in the
population) value and its respective confidence interval (C.I. – 95%) of
copper to shrimps were calculated by using PritProbit 1.63 (Sakuma,
1998) from the mean avoidance percentage (mean from the exposure
times of 1, 2 and 3 h) observed for each copper concentration.
3. Results
3.1. Control experiments with no copper: the organisms' distribution
3.1.1. Experiment A
The distribution of the organisms among the three zones (after the
Greenhouse correction as sphericity was not assumed; Table S1a –
Supplementary material) did not vary with time (F = 0.009; P =
0.998) nor for the interaction time ∗ compartment (F = 1.537; P =
0.231; Table S1b – Supplementary material). Regarding the percentage
of organisms in each zone (34.8 ± 7.7%, 25.0 ± 5.6% and 40.1 ± 4.4% for
the zones R1, R2 and R3, respectively – Fig. 3), no statistically significant
difference was observed (F = 4.998; P = 0.053; Table S1c – Supplementary material). Therefore, this control experiment was considered acceptable because there was no a trend to avoid the extremities (this
behavior could be critical for the copper gradient experiments).
3.1.2. Experiment B
The distribution of the organisms among the three zones throughout
the time (sphericity was assumed: Table S2a – Supplementary material)
did not vary with time (F = 0.080; P = 0.995) but did for the interaction
time ∗ compartment (F = 2.615; P = 0.020; Table S2b – Supplementary
material). Regarding the percentage of organisms (25.6 ± 5.3%, 35.5 ±
3.5% and 38.9 ± 1.9% for the zones R1, R2 and R3, respectively – Fig. 3),
ANOVA detected a statistically different distribution of the organisms (F
= 9.603; P = 0.013; Table S2c – Supplementary material), although this
difference was not detected by the Bonferronni test (Table S2d - Supplementary material). The control was considered acceptable because
there was no a trend to avoid the extremities (this behavior could be
critical for the copper gradient experiments).
820
V.C. Vera-Vera et al. / Science of the Total Environment 662 (2019) 816–823
Fig. 3. Mean percentages (n = 6 exposure times; and standard deviation) of the shrimps Atyaephyra desmarestii found in different zones (R1, R2 and R3) in the experiments A, B and C
(control experiments with culture water) during 3 h exposure. Similar letters indicate no statistically significant difference by the Bonferroni test (significance: P b 0.05).
3.1.3. Experiment C
The distribution of the organisms among the three zones (after the
Greenhouse correction as sphericity was not assumed; Table S3a – Supplementary material) did not vary with time (F = 0.049; P = 0.966) nor
for the interaction time ∗ compartment (F = 1.185; P = 0.364)
(Table S3b – Supplementary material). Regarding the percentage of organisms (34.9 ± 6.3%, 33.6 ± 3.9% 31.5 ± 3.3% for the zones R1, R2 and
R3, respectively – Fig. 3), the organisms' distribution was not statistically different (F = 0.177; P = 0.842; Table S3c – Material supplementary). This control was also considered acceptable as no trend to avoid
the extremes was evidenced.
Fig. 4), the results showed no statistically significant difference (F =
0.286; P = 0.761; Table S5c – Supplementary material).
3.2.3. Experiment F
No mortality was recorded during this experiment. The distribution
of the organisms among the three zones (considering the Greenhouse
correction as sphericity was not assumed; Table S6a – Supplementary
material) did not vary with time (F = 0.029; P = 0.997) nor for the interaction time ∗ compartment (F = 1.351; P = 0.276) (Table S6b – Supplementary material). However, statistically significant differences (F
= 28.402; P b 0.001; Table S6c – Supplementary material) were observed for the distribution of organisms (44.9 ± 4.8%, 33.2 ± 5.1% and
21.9 ± 0.5% for the zones R, M and D, respectively – Fig. 4).
3.2. Experiments with copper
4. Discussion
3.2.1. Experiment D
No mortality was recorded during this experiment. The distribution
of the organisms among the three zones (after the Greenhouse correction as sphericity was not assumed; Table S4a – Supplementary material) did not vary with time (F = 0.084; P = 0.975) nor for the
interaction time ∗ compartment (F = 1.502; P = 0.226) (Table S4b –
Supplementary material). The percentage of organisms (59.6 ± 8.0%,
28.9 ± 3.5% and 11.6 ± 4.5% for the zones R, M and D, respectively –
Fig. 4) was statistically different (F = 53.414; P b 0.0001; Table S4c –
Material supplementary) between the three zones. The mean avoidance
of the population [AC50 = 70.1 (55.3–96.6) μg/L] was calculated based
on the avoidance percentage and considering the copper concentrations
in each compartment (not by zones) (Fig. 5).
3.2.2. Experiment E
No mortality was recorded during this experiment. The distribution
of the organisms among the three zones, after the Greenhouse correction (sphericity was not assumed; Table S5a – Supplementary material),
did not vary with time (F = 0.029; P = 0.996) nor for the interaction
time ∗ compartment (F = 1.374; P = 0.269) (Table S5b – Supplementary material). Regarding the percentage of organisms (34.1 ± 8.9%,
35.6 ± 7.9% and 30.2 ± 10.2% for the zones R, M and D, respectively –
The present study simulated a linear copper gradient and two heterogeneous environments with patches contaminated by copper in
the HeMHAS to check if the freshwater shrimp A. desmarestii was able
to detect and avoid contamination. The initial results of the control experiments (experiments A, B and C) indicated that the HeMHAS and
the laboratory conditions did not affect the habitat selection by shrimps
as the organisms were randomly distributed in the absence of contamination. A previous study using the HeMHAS as the test system, in which
the fish Danio rerio was exposed to a copper gradient, also showed the
suitability of the HeMHAS as an exposure system, as it caused no interference in the displacement of the organisms (Araújo et al., 2018b).
Checking this randomness is crucial for the interpretation of the avoidance response in multi-compartmented systems and ensuring that no
confounding factor is interfering in the selection of zones by the organisms (Lopes et al., 2004; Moreira-Santos et al., 2008; Silva et al., 2018).
When the organisms were exposed to the copper gradient (experiment D), a clear avoidance was observed such as previously described
by other authors using different test systems and organisms (Hartwell
et al., 1989; Svecevičius, 1999; Richardson et al., 2001; Lefcort et al.,
2004; see also review by Araújo et al., 2016a). Using other multicompartmented linear systems, avoidance to copper has been also evidenced for different species, such: as the cladocera Daphnia longispina
Fig. 4. Mean percentages (n = 12 exposure times; and standard deviation) of the shrimps Atyaephyra desmarestii found in different zones (R, M and D) in the experiments D, E and F
(avoidance experiments with copper) during 3 h exposure. Different letters indicate statistically significant differences by the Bonferroni test (significance: P b 0.05).
V.C. Vera-Vera et al. / Science of the Total Environment 662 (2019) 816–823
Fig. 5. Mean percentage (from exposure periods of 1, 2 and 3 h) of avoidance by the
shrimp Atyaephyra desmarestii exposed to a linear copper gradient.
(Lopes et al., 2004), the marine shrimp Litopenaeus vannamei (Araújo
et al., 2016c), tadpoles of the amphibians Leptodactylus latrans,
Lithobates catesbeianus and Pelophylax perezi (Araújo et al., 2014b), the
freshwater fish D. rerio (Moreira-Santos et al., 2008; Islam et al., 2019)
and Poecilia reticulata (Silva et al., 2018) and the marine fish
Rachycentron canadum (Araújo et al., 2016c). The sensitivity of
A. desmarestii to avoid copper (AC50 around 70 μg/L) is comparable to
the avoidance response to copper observed by other authors that reported AC50 for cladocerans and fish varying from around 15 to 180
μg/L (Moreira-Santos et al., 2008; Silva et al., 2018; Islam et al., 2019;
see also the review by Araújo and Blasco, 2019). The avoidance response
to copper in the HeMHAS has only been described for D. rerio until now,
which responded in a similar way to A. desmarestii, with an AC50 of 60
μg/L (Araújo et al., 2018b). In any experiments, neither erratic movements, grouping behavior nor signals of lethargy were observed in the
shrimps. Behavioral changes as torpidity or moribundity due to exposure to copper can occur when high concentrations (from 200 μg/L)
are used such as have been described for tadpoles (Araújo et al., 2014b).
The use of the HeMHAS has made it possible to simulate a more heterogeneous scenario of contamination as concentrations could be arranged in patches, such as in experiments E and F. If, on the one hand,
it was expected that under a marked linear gradient the shrimps
would move to less contaminated zones, on the other hand, in a patchy
contamination scenario a random distribution was expected due to the
higher possibility of finding less contaminated zones, which would lead
to a higher displacement. This hypothesis was supported by the data
from experiment E (no clear preference for the less contaminated
zones was observed), but not for experiment F (less contaminated
zones were preferred). Although the shrimps tended to avoid the contamination by copper, the randomness of the behavior regarding habitat
preference observed in experiment E highlights the importance of clean
zones to the dispersion dynamics of the populations. Due to an almost
continuous observation (12 observations during the 3 h period), the
preference behavior cannot be attributed to chance. Since ecotoxicology
is not limited to assessing toxic effects, but also tries to understand and
explain changes in the dynamic of populations driven by contamination
(Jepson and Sherratt, 1996; Spromberg et al., 1998; Exley, 2000;
Chapman, 2002), identifying potential effects on the dispersion pattern
of organisms might be a first step to prevent alterations in the community, such as the partial or total disappearance of a population due to its
fleeing towards other areas (Lopes et al., 2004; Scott and Sloman, 2004;
Rosa et al., 2012; Moe et al., 2013). In fact, to improve the ecological aspect of ecotoxicological studies, the concepts of colonization and recolonization of recovering habitats (Van den Brink, 2008; Araújo et al.,
2018d; Islam et al., 2019), habitat fragmentation due to a chemical barrier (Fuller et al., 2015; Araújo et al., 2018e, 2019) and metapopulation
821
(assessing the quality of connected habitats, the dynamics and persistence of populations – Grimm et al., 2003) a should be more frequently
used.
The HeMHAS represents, therefore, a novel approach to studying the
risk linked to environmental contamination by which it is possible to assess avoidance and preference responses in a patchy and more complex
contamination scenario. The simulation of the heterogeneity of habitat
makes it possible to assess how contamination might condition the dispersion of the organisms and their habitat selection processes. Although
the traditional ecotoxicity tests are not able to simulate spatial heterogeneity due to the forced exposure scenario (Chapman, 2002; Roberts
et al., 2008), the non-forced, multi-compartmented systems, such as
the HeMHAS, help to increase the ecological relevance of environmental
risk assessment studies by providing a complementary tool to ecotoxicology (Araújo et al., 2018b). This relevance is applicable for mobile organisms and scenarios in which an environmental heterogeneity exists.
The environmental heterogeneity occurs in time and space very frequently (Palmer et al., 2010), therefore, ecotoxicological studies should
integrate this factor not only by performing seasonal sampling (temporal
heterogeneity) in different areas (spatial heterogeneity), but also by considering the habitat connectivity and the possible displacement of organisms between habitats (Spromberg et al., 1998; Fleeger et al., 2003). The
reduction or loss of connectivity between habitats due to contamination
might produce important changes in the ecosystems regarding: the biological interactions (Branco et al., 2012; Hurd et al., 2016), reduction in
the resilience and resistance of the ecosystems (Schmitt-Jansen et al.,
2008; Clements and Rohr, 2009) and in the genetic diversity (Roark
et al., 2005; Ribeiro and Lopes, 2013), restrictions of habitable areas
(Saunders and Sprague, 1967), alterations in the migratory routes
(Gray, 1990; Smith and Bailey, 1990) and increases in the vulnerability
of the community (Atchison et al., 1987; Moe et al., 2013).
Due to the heterogeneity of the habitat quality, patchy environments
have been considered of great importance to the maintenance of populations as they favor the displacement dynamics of organisms (Wu et al.,
1993; Fuller et al., 2015). In this sense, the mixing zones of the present
study (named zone M - moderately contaminated) might have played
an important role as zones tolerable by organisms and might have
served, just like the clean zones, to minimize the deleterious effects of
the contaminants, favoring the random dispersion of organisms
(Sherratt and Jepson, 1993; Maurer and Holt, 1996). Zones functioning
like this might play an important role for the interactions and flows between ecosystems in a meta-ecosystem arrangement (Loreau et al.,
2003). Habitat heterogeneity, regarding contamination, should be considered with special interest in ecotoxicology because the stability and
recovery of some ecosystems depend to some extent (not exclusively)
on the environmental heterogeneity (Palmer et al., 2010). Finally, it is
important to highlight that the habitat heterogeneity does not imply a
reduced risk of contamination as organisms tend to displace randomly
between areas, instead, the present study shows the importance of preserving clean areas to guarantee the presence of at least some refuges
where organisms can avoid the toxic effects of contamination.
Acknowledgements
V. Vera-Vera thanks The Postgraduate Iberoamerican University Association (AUIP) for the Master's fellowship. C.V.M. Araújo is grateful to
the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities for a Juan
de la Cierva contract (IJCI-2014-19318). The authors thank David Roque
and Antonio Moreno their technical assistance in the fieldwork and Jon
Nesbit for his revision of the English text. This study was partially
funded by the Explora project (#CGL2017-92160-EXP).
Appendix A. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2019.01.304.
822
V.C. Vera-Vera et al. / Science of the Total Environment 662 (2019) 816–823
References
Araújo, C.V.M., Blasco, J., 2019. Spatial avoidance as a response to contamination by
aquatic organisms in non-forced, multi-compartmented exposure systems: a complementary approach to the behavioral response. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. https://
doi.org/10.1002/etc.4310 (in press).
Araújo, C.V.M., Blasco, J., Moreno-Garrido, I., 2012. Measuring the avoidance behaviour shown by the snail Hydrobia ulvae exposed to sediment with a known contamination gradient. Ecotoxicology 21, 750–758. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10646-011-0835-6.
Araújo, C.V.M., Moreira-Santos, M., Sousa, J.P., Ochoa-Herrera, V., Encalada, A.C., Ribeiro,
R., 2014a. Active avoidance from a crude oil soluble fraction by an Andean paramo copepod. Ecotoxicology 23, 1254–1259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10646-014-1268-9.
Araújo, C.V.M., Shinn, C., Moreira-Santos, M., Lopes, I., Espíndola, E.L.G., Ribeiro, R., 2014b.
Copper-driven avoidance and mortality in temperate and tropical tadpoles. Aquat.
Toxicol. 146, 70–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquatox.2013.10.030.
Araújo, C.V.M., Moreira-Santos, M., Ribeiro, R., 2016a. Active and passive spatial avoidance
by aquatic organisms from environmental stressors: a complementary perspective
and a critical review. Environ. Int. 92, 405–415. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
envint.2016.04.031.
Araújo, C.V.M., Rodríguez, E.N.V., Salvatierra, D., Cedeño-Macias, L.A., Vera-Vera, V.C.,
Moreira-Santos, M., Ribeiro, R., 2016b. Attractiveness of food and avoidance from
contamination as conflicting stimuli to habitat selection by fish. Chemosphere 163,
177–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chemosphere.2016.08.029.
Araújo, C.V.M., Cedeño-Macías, L.A., Vera-Vera, V.C., Salvatierra, D., Rodríguez, E.N.V.,
Zambrano, U., Kuri, S., 2016c. Predicting the effects of copper on local population decline of 2 marine organisms, cobia fish and whiteleg shrimp, based on avoidance response. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 35, 405–410. https://doi.org/10.1002/etc.3192.
Araújo, C.V.M., Griffith, D.M., Vera-Vera, V.C., Jentzsch, P.V., Cervera, L., Nieto-Ariza, B.,
Salvatierra, D., Erazo, S., Jaramillo, R., Ramos, L.A., Moreira-Santos, M., Ribeiro, R.,
2018a. A novel approach to assessing environmental disturbance based on habitat selection by zebra fish as a model organism. Sci. Total Environ. 619–620, 906–915.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2017.11.170.
Araújo, C.V.M., Roque, D., Blasco, J., Ribeiro, R., Moreira-Santos, M., Toribio, A., Aguirre, E.,
Barro, S., 2018b. Stress-driven emigration in complex field scenarios of habitat disturbance: the Heterogeneous Multi-Habitat Assay System (HeMHAS). Sci. Total Environ.
644, 31–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2018.06.336.
Araújo, C.V.M., Pereira, K.C., Blasco, J., 2018c. Avoidance response by shrimps to a copper
gradient: does high population density prevent avoidance of contamination? Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 37, 3095–3101. https://doi.org/10.1002/etc.4277.
Araújo, C.V.M., Moreira-Santos, M., Ribeiro, R., 2018d. Stressor-driven emigration and
recolonisation patterns in disturbed habitats. Sci. Total Environ. 643, 884–889.
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.scitotenv.2018.06.264.
Araújo, C.V.M., Silva, D.C.V.R., Gomes, L.E.T., Acayaba, R.D., Montagner, C.C., MoreiraSantos, M., Ribeiro, R., Pompêo, M.L.M., 2018e. Habitat fragmentation caused by contaminants: atrazine as a chemical barrier isolating fish populations. Chemosphere
193, 24–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chemosphere.2017.11.014.
Araújo, C.V.M., González-Ortegón, E., Pintado-Herrera, M., Biel-Maeso, M., Lara-Martín, P.,
Tovar-Sánchez, A., Blasco, J., 2019. Disturbance of ecological habitat distribution
driven by a chemical barrier of domestic and agricultural discharges: an experimental
approach to test habitat fragmentation. Sci. Total Environ. 651, 2820–2829. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2018.10.200.
Atchison, G.J., Henry, M.G., Sendheinrich, M.B., 1987. Effects of metal on fish behavior: a
review. Environ. Biol. Fish 18, 11–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00002324.
Ǻtland, Ǻ., Barlaup, B.T., 1995. Avoidance of toxic mixing zones by Atlantic salmon (Salmo
salar L.) and brown trout (Salmo trutta L.) in the limed river Audna, southern Norway.
Environ. Pollut. 90, 203–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/0269-7491(95)00002-9.
Branco, P., Segurado, P., Santos, J.M., Pinheiro, P., Ferreira, M.T., 2012. Does longitudinal
connectivity loss affect the distribution of freshwater fish? Ecol. Eng. 48, 70–78.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoleng.2011.05.008.
Calow, P., 1989. The choice and implementation of environmental bioassays.
Hydrobiologia 188, 61–64. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00027771.
Capillo, G., Silvestro, S., Sanfilippo, M., Fiorino, E., Giangrosso, G., Ferrantelli, V., Vazzana, I.,
Faggio, C., 2018. Assessment of electrolytes and metals profile of the Faro Lake (Capo
Peloro Lagoon, Sicily, Italy) and its impact on Mytilus galloprovincialis. Chem.
Biodivers. 15, 1800044. https://doi.org/10.1002/cbdv.201800044.
Chapman, P.M., 2002. Integrating toxicology and ecology: putting the “eco” into ecotoxicology. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 44, 7–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0025-326X(01)00253-3.
Clements, W.H., Rohr, J.R., 2009. Community responses to contaminants: using basic ecological principles to predict ecotoxicological effects. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 28,
1789–1800. https://doi.org/10.1897/09-140.1.
De Lange, H.J., Sperber, V., Peeters, E., 2006. Avoidance of polycyclic aromatic hydocarbon
contaminated sediment by the freshwater invertebrates Gammarus pulex and Asellus
aquaticus. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 25, 452–457. https://doi.org/10.1897/05-413.1.
Ewers, R.M., Thorpe, S., Didham, R.K., 2007. Synergistic interactions between edge and
area effects in a heavily fragmented landscape. Ecology 88, 96–106. https://doi.org/
10.1890/0012-9658(2007)88[96:SIBEAA]2.0.CO;2.
Exley, C., 2000. Avoidance of aluminum by rainbow trout. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 19,
933–939. https://doi.org/10.1002/etc.5620190421.
Faggio, C., Pagano, M., Alampi, R., Vazzana, I., Felice, M.R., 2016. Cytotoxicity,
haemolymphatic parameters, and oxidative stress following exposure to sub-lethal
concentrations of quaternium-15 in Mytilus galloprovincialis. Aquat. Toxicol. 180,
258–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquatox.2016.10.010.
Fidalgo, M.L., Gerhardt, A., 2002. Distribution of the freshwater shrimp, Atyaephyra
desmaresti (Millet, 1831) in Portugal (Decapoda, Natantia). Crustaceana 75,
1375–1385. https://doi.org/10.1163/156854002321629808.
Fleeger, J.W., Carman, K.R., Nisbet, R.M., 2003. Indirect effects of contaminants in aquatic
ecosystems. Sci. Total Environ. 317, 207–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0048-9697
(03)00141-4.
Folmar, L.C., 1976. Overt avoidance reaction of rainbow trout fry to nine herbicides. Bull.
Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 15, 509–514.
Fuller, M.R., Doyle, M.W., Strayer, D.L., 2015. Causes and consequences of habitat fragmentation in river networks. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1355, 31–51. https://doi.org/10.1111/
nyas.12853.
Gray, R.H., 1990. Fish behaviour and environmental assessment. Environ. Toxicol. Chem.
9, 53–67. https://doi.org/10.1002/etc.5620090108.
Grimm, V., Reise, K., Strasser, M., 2003. Marine metapopulations: a useful concept? Helgol.
Mar. Res. 56, 222–228. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10152-002-0121-3.
Gunn, J.M., Noakes, D.L.G., 1986. Avoidance of low pH and elevated Al concentrations by
brook charr (Salvelinus fontinalis) alevins in laboratory. Water Air Soil Pollut. 30,
497–503. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00305218.
Hansen, J.A., Marr, J.C.A., Lipton, J., Cacela, D., Bergman, H.L., 1999a. Differences in neurobehavioral responses of Chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha) and rainbow
trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) exposed to copper and cobalt: behavioral avoidance. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 18, 1972–1978. https://doi.org/10.1002/etc.5620180916.
Hansen, J.A., Woorward, D.F., Little, E.E., DeLonay, A.J., Bergman, H.L., 1999b. Behavioral
avoidance: possible mechanism for explaining abundance and distribution of trout
species in a metal-impacted river. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 18, 313–317. https://doi.
org/10.1002/etc.5620180231.
Hartwell, S.I., Jin, J.H., Cherry, D.S., Cairns Jr., J., 1989. Toxicity versus avoidance response of
golden shiner, Notemigonus crysoleucas, to five metals. J. Fish Biol. 35, 447–456.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.1989.tb02995.x.
Hellou, J., 2011. Beavioural ecotoxicology, an “early warning” signal to assess environmental quality. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 18, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11356010-0367-2.
Hurd, L.E., Sousa, R.G.C., Siqueira-Souza, F.K., Cooper, G.J., Kahn, J.R., Freitas, C.E.C., 2016.
Amazon floodplain fish communities: habitat connectivity and conservation in a rapidly deteriorating environment. Biol. Conserv. 195, 118–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.biocon.2016.01.005.
Islam, M.A., Blasco, J., Araújo, C.V.M., 2019. Spatial avoidance, inhibition of recolonization
and population isolation in zebrafish (Danio rerio) caused by copper exposure under
a non-forced approach. Sci. Total Environ. 653, 504–511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
scitotenv.2018.10.375.
Jepson, P.C., Sherratt, T.N., 1996. The dimensions of space and time in the assessment of
ecotoxicological risks. In: Baird, D.J., Maltby, L., Greig-Smith, P.W., Douben, P.E.T.
(Eds.), ECOtoxicology: Ecological Dimensions. Champman & Hall Ecotoxicology Series. Springer, Dordrecht, pp. 43–54 https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-1541-1_5.
Jones, J.R.E., 1947. The reaction of Pygosteus pungitius L. to toxic solutions. J. Exp. Biol. 24,
110–122.
Jones, J.R.E., 1948. A further study of the reaction of fish to toxic solutions. Water quality
criteria, 1972. J. Exp. Biol. 25, 22–34.
Khan, F.R., 2018. Ecotoxicology in the Anthropocene: are we listening to nature's scream?
Environ. Sci. Technol. 52, 10227–10229. https://doi.org/10.1021/acs.est.8b04534.
Kimball, K.D., Levin, S.A., 1985. Limitations of laboratory bioassays: the need for
ecosystem-level testing. Bioscience 35, 165–171. https://doi.org/10.2307/1309866.
Lefcort, H., Abbott, D.P., Cleary, D.A., Howell, D., Keller, N.C., Smith, M.M., 2004. Aquatic
snails from mining sites have evolved to detect and avoid heavy metals. Arch. Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 46, 478–484. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00244-003-3029-2.
Lopes, I., Baird, D.J., Ribeiro, R., 2004. Avoidance of copper contamination by field populations of Daphnia longispina. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 23, 1702–1708. https://doi.org/
10.1897/03-231.
Loreau, M., Mouquet, N., Holt, R.D., 2003. Meta-ecosystems: a theoretical framework for a
spatial ecosystem ecology. Ecol. Lett. 6, 673–679. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.14610248.2003.00483.x.
Maltby, L., Calow, P., 1989. The application of bioassays in the resolution of environmental
problems; past, present and future. Hydrobiologia 188–189, 65–76. https://doi.org/
10.1007/978-94-009-1896-2_5.
Maurer, B.A., Holt, R.D., 1996. Effects of chronic pesticide stress on wildlife populations in
complex landscapes: processes at multiple scales. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 15,
420–426. https://doi.org/10.1002/etc.5620150403.
Millet, P.A., 1831. Description d'une nouvelle espèce de Crustacé, l'Hippolyte de
Desmarest. memoires de la société d'Agriculture. Sci. Arts d'Angers. 1, 55–57.
Moe, S.J., De Schamphelaere, K., Clements, W.H., Sorensen, M.T., Van den Brink, P.J., Liess,
M., 2013. Combined and interactive effects of global climate change and toxicants on
populations and communities. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 32, 49–61. https://doi.org/
10.1002/etc.2045.
Moog, O., Nesemann, H., Itek, A., Melcher, A., 1999. Erstnachweis der süßwassergarnele
Atyaephyra desmaresti (Millet, 1831) (Decapoda) in Österreich. Lauterbornia 35,
67–70. https://www.zobodat.at/pdf/Lauterbornia_1999_35_0067-0070.pdf.
Moreira-Santos, M., Donato, C., Lopes, I., Ribeiro, R., 2008. Avoidance test with small fish:
determination of the median avoidance concentration and of the lowest-observedeffect gradient. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 27, 1576–1582. https://doi.org/10.1897/07094.1.
Palmer, M.A., Menninger, H.L., Bernhardt, E., 2010. River restoration, habitat heterogeneity and biodiversity: a failure of theory or practice? Freshw. Biol. 55, 205–222. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2427.2009.02372.x.
Ribeiro, R., Lopes, I., 2013. Contaminant driven genetic erosion and associated hypotheses
on alleles loss, reduced population growth rate and increased susceptibility to future
stressors: an essay. Ecotoxicology 22, 889–899. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10646-0131070-0.
Richardson, J., Williams, E.K., Hickey, C.W., 2001. Avoidance behaviour of freshwater fish
and shrimp exposed to ammonia and dissolved oxygen separately and in
V.C. Vera-Vera et al. / Science of the Total Environment 662 (2019) 816–823
combination. N. Z. J. Mar. Freshw. Res. 35, 625–633. https://doi.org/10.1080/
00288330.2001.9517028.
Roark, S.A., Nacci, D., Coiro, L., Champlin, D., Guttman, S.I., 2005. Population genetic structure of a nonmigratory estuarine fish (Fundulus heteroclitus) across a strong gradient
of polychlorinated biphenyl contamination. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 24, 717–725.
https://doi.org/10.1897/03-687.1.
Roberts, D.A., Johnston, E.L., Müller, S., Poore, A.G.B., 2008. Field and laboratory simulations of storm water pulses: behavioural avoidance by marine epifauna. Environ.
Pollut. 152, 153–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envpol.2007.05.005.
Rosa, R., Materatski, P., Moreira-Santos, M., Sousa, J.P., Ribeiro, R., 2012. A scaled-up system to evaluate zooplankton spatial avoidance and population immediate decline
concentration. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 31, 1301–1305. https://doi.org/10.1002/
etc.1813.
Sakuma, M., 1998. Probrit analysis of preference data. Appl. Entomol. Zool. (3), 339–347
https://doi.org/10.1303/aez.33.339.
Saunders, R.L., Sprague, J.B., 1967. Effect of copper-zinc mining pollution on a spawning
migration of Atlantic salmon. Water Res. 1, 419–432. https://doi.org/10.1016/00431354(67)90051-6.
Schmitt-Jansen, M., Veit, U., Dudel, G., Altenburger, R., 2008. An ecological perspective in
aquatic ecotoxicology: approaches and challenges. Basic Appl. Ecol. 9, 337–345.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.baae.2007.08.008.
Scott, G.R., Sloman, K.A., 2004. The effects of environmental pollutants on complex fish
behaviour: integrating behavioural and physiological indicators of toxicity. Aquat.
Toxicol. 68, 369–392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquatox.2004.03.016.
Segner, H., Schmitt-Jansen, M., Sabater, S., 2014. Assessing the impact of multiple stressors
on aquatic biota: the receptor's side matters. Environ. Sci. Technol. 48, 7690–7696.
https://doi.org/10.1021/es405082t.
Sherratt, T.N., Jepson, P.C., 1993. A metapopulation approach to modeling the long-term
impact of pesticides on invertebrates. J. Appl. Ecol. 30, 696–705. https://doi.org/
10.2307/2404248.
Silva, D.C.V., Araújo, C.V.M., López-Doval, J.C., Neto, M.B., Silva, F.T., Paiva, T.C.B., Ponpêo,
M.L.M., 2017. Potential effects of triclosan on spatial displacement and local population decline of the fish Poecilia reticulata using a non-forced system. Chemosphere
184, 329–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chemosphere.2017.06.002.
Silva, D.C.V.R., Araújo, C.V.M., França, F.M., Neto, M.B., Paiva, T.C.B., Silva, F.T., Pampêo,
M.L.M., 2018. Bisphenol risk in fish exposed to a contamination gradient: triggering
of spatial avoidance. Aquat. Toxicol. 197 (1–6). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
aquatox.2018.01.016.
823
Smith, E.H., Bailey, H.C., 1990. Preference/avoidance testing of waste discharges on anadromous fish. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 9, 77–86. https://doi.org/10.1002/
etc.5620090110.
Spromberg, J.A., John, B.M., Landis, W.G., 1998. Metapopulation dynamics: indirect effects
and multiple distinct outcomes in ecological risk assessment. Environ. Toxicol. Chem.
17, 1640–1649. https://doi.org/10.1002/etc.5620170828.
Straalen, N.M., 2003. Ecotoxicology becomes stress ecology. Viewpoint. Environ. Sci.
Technol. 37, 324–330. https://doi.org/10.1021/es0325720.
Svecevičius, G., 1999. Fish avoidance response to heavy metals and their mixtures. Act.
Zoo. Lituan. 9, 103–113. https://doi.org/10.1080/13921657.1999.10512293.
Tierney, K.B., 2016. Chemical avoidance responses of fish. Aquat. Toxicol. 174, 228–241.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aquatox.2016.02.021.
Tierney, K.B., Baldwin, D.H., Hara, T.J., Ross, P.S., Scholz, N.L., Kennedy, C.J., 2010. Olfactory
toxicity in fish. Aquat. Toxicol. 96, 2–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
aquatox.2009.09.019.
Tittizer, T., Schöll, F., Banning, A., Haybach, A., Schleuter, M., 2000. Aquatische neozoen
makrozoobenthos der Binnenwasserstraben en Deutchlands. Lauterbornia 39, 1–72.
https://www.zobodat.at/pdf/Lauterbornia_2000_39_0001-0072.pdf.
Van den Brink, P.J., 2008. Ecological risk assessment: from book-keeping to chemical
stress ecology. Environ. Sci. Technol. 42, 8999–9004. https://doi.org/10.1021/
es801991c.
Van den Brink, F.W.B., Van Der Velde, G., 1986. Observation on the seasonal and yearly occurrence and the distribution of Atyaephyra desmaresti (Millet, 1831) (Crustacea,
Decapoda, Natantia) in the Netherlands. Hydrobiol. Bull. 19, 193–198. https://doi.
org/10.1007/BF02270766.
Vasconcelos, A.M., Daam, M.A., dos Santos, L.R.A., Sanches, A.L.M., Araújo, C.V.M.,
Espíndola, E.L.G., 2016. Acute and chronic sensitivity, avoidance behavior and sensitive life stages of bullfrog tadpoles exposed to the biopesticide abamectin. Ecotoxicology 25, 500–509. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10646-015-1608-4.
Wells, J.B., Little, E.E., Calfee, R.D., 2004. Behavioral response of young rainbow trout
(Oncorhynchus mykiss) to forest fire-retardant chemicals in the laboratory. Environ.
Toxicol. Chem. 23, 621–625. https://doi.org/10.1897/02-635.
Wu, J., Vankat, J.L., Barlas, Y., 1993. Effects of patch connectivity and arrangement on animal metapopulation dynamics: a simulation study. Ecol. Model. 65, 221–254.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3800(93)90081-3.
Descargar