Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 98: 345–351, 2001. © 2001 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. 345 Prostephanus truncatus mate choice on contact: does pheromone signalling by males affect their mating success? L. A. Birkinshaw1 & R. H. Smith2 1 Natural Resources Institute, University of Greenwich, Chatham Maritime, Kent ME4 4TB, UK (E-mail: [email protected]); 2 School of Biological Sciences, University of Leicester, Leicester LE1 7RH, UK (E-mail: [email protected]) Accepted: October 10, 2000 Key words: Bostrichidae, Prostephanus truncatus, aggregation pheromone, sexual selection, female mate choice Abstract Prostephanus truncatus is an economically important beetle pest of stored maize and cassava in the tropics. Male beetles signal using an aggregation pheromone that attracts both female and male beetles over large distances. Females preferentially orientate towards the pheromone signals of particular males when given a choice. The influence of pheromone signalling on courtship and mating success was investigated using pheromone biossays and mating trials in both of which a female made a choice between two males. Signalling was manipulated by exposing males to a Female Factor that inhibits pheromone production. The relative attractiveness of males to females based on pheromone bioassays was found not to influence short-range courtship behaviour or mating success. Introduction Prostephanus truncatus (Coleoptera: Bostrichidae) is a destructive pest of stored maize and cassava in the tropics and is commonly known as the Larger Grain Borer or LGB (Hodges, 1996). LGB shows many of the characteristics of wood-boring beetles and will form complex tunnel systems with main tunnels and blind-ending tunnels when offered a suitable host (Li, 1988). Aggregations of beetles form in response to signalling by males releasing an aggregation pheromone that attracts both sexes over a long range (tens or 100s of metres) (Scholz et al., 1997). Traps baited with synthetic aggregation pheromone are used to monitor both the population dynamics and the spatial distribution of LGB (Borgemeister et al., 1997a,b). The hypothesis that males signal primarily to attract females (Hodges, 1994) is supported by three observations: (1) Only the males signal (Cork et al., 1991). (2) The sex ratio of beetles attracted by males in the field is female-biased (Scholz et al., 1997). (3) Males stop emitting pheromone signals when females are present (Smith et al., 1996). Birkinshaw & Smith (2000) investigated the role of aggregation pheromone in sexual selection in LGB using olfactometer and field tests, and showed that both males and females respond consistently and preferentially to the signals emitted by some individuals in choice tests. Their results support the hypothesis that LGB aggregationpheromone signals are sexually selected. The males that respond to the pheromone may be exploiting the efforts and ability of the signaller to attract females, and the signalling male may suffer a fitness cost of increased competition for both mates and host resources. Males decrease the rate of production of pheromone within 24 h of exposure to live females and they respond in a similar way to an unknown, non-volatile substance known as ‘Female Factor’ left in grain that has been infested by females (Smith et al., 1996). The balance of the costs and benefits of signalling will be determined in part by the short-range effect of aggregation pheromone on courtship behaviour and mating success. Adult LGB that encounter each other in an open mating arena often push each other, and this behaviour is especially prevalent when a female comes into contact with a male, suggesting that push- 346 ing may be part of courtship. Females appear to be generally reluctant to mate. Pushing behaviour may enable beetles to assess aspects of quality, prevent copulation, or serve to stimulate further sexual behaviour (Birkinshaw, 1998). Males also push other males and this may be a form of intrasexual selection, carried out either to increase access to mates or to defend resources such as tunnel systems. It is not known, however, whether pushing behaviour is related to either body size or pheromone production. Male size is often a good predictor of mating success in arthropods (Crespi, 1989; Andersson, 1994). Small males may be more manoeuvrable and agile than large males and this could help in obtaining mates in species that copulate in flight or in water. Large males may perform best if there is visual or physical assessment of ‘quality’, either within or between sexes. Females may produce higher quality offspring by selecting larger mates if body size and fecundity are genetically correlated (Holloway & Smith, 1987). In the cockroach Nauphoeta cinerea, long-range pheromones have been found to influence short-range courtship behaviour (Moore, 1998; Moore et al., 1995). Placing pheromone-impregnated discs of filter paper on the backs of male cockroaches elevated their dominance status during courtship (Moore et al., 1997). It is not known whether LGB courtship is also influenced by aggregation pheromones. Thus the overall question about LGB behaviour addressed here is as follows: is a male who is more attractive than other males at a distance more successful than those other males in short-range interactions of courtship and mating? In order to address this question, we present results from three approaches: (1) comparison of courtship behaviour in relation to mating success, (2) comparison of relative attractiveness of two males to a female in pheromone bioassays and their performance in mating trials, (3) manipulation of pheromone signal by exposing one male of a pair to grain impregnated with Female Factor. Materials and methods All insects used were from a laboratory culture originally established using LGB collected in Tanzania during the 1980s and maintained on whole maize at the Natural Resources Institute (NRI), UK. The maize used in experiments was ‘Natco medium corn meal’ supplied by T. Cholthram and sons Ltd, Middlesex, UK. Both insect cultures and experiments were kept in a controlled temperature and humidity (CTH) room at 30 ◦ C and 70% r.h. with a L12:D12 light regime. Experimental insects were adults of mixed ages randomly selected from sieved cultures. Individual beetles were sexed (by examining genitalia; Birkinshaw, 1998), placed singly on medium-ground maize in a 36 cm3 glass pot, and held for 12 days. Males were weighed using a Cahn electrobalance 5 days after they were sexed. Pheromone bioassays. An olfactometer was used to assess the preference of a single female given a choice of signals from two males. The apparatus is described by Birkinshaw & Smith (2000) and consists of a circular central arena (diameter 9.5 cm) in which the walking response of a beetle (the responder) to four plumes of air is recorded. Two of the plumes of air were passed over control pots containing only food (ground maize) and the other two plumes were passed over experimental pots each containing a single male on food. Each female therefore had a direct choice between volatiles emitted by two different males. Bioassays measured the number of times the head and prothorax of the responder was placed in each tube carrying an air plume from a pot during a 20-min period of observation; the responder was placed back in the centre of the arena after each response (Birkinshaw & Smith, 2000). Mating trials. The open mating arena was a Petri dish (9.5 cm diameter) with its floor lined with filter paper. Two males were placed with a single female in the arena and courtship behaviour was observed for 30 min, or until a male copulated with the female. The identity of the male that copulated was noted. Courtship behaviour. Three measures were recorded for each physical contact between any two test-beetles: (1) the identity of the beetle that approached another beetle, (2) the identity of the beetle being approached, (3) the identity of the pushing beetle (when a push was observed). The identity of the beetle that initiated an encounter was recorded as an indication of active pursuit of mates. The strength of the push was not quantified. 347 Experiment 1. Natural variation: does long-range attraction predict the progress and outcome of shortrange courtship? Each replicate consisted of one female with two males. Males were chosen to differ in weight by 0.4–0.5 mg. Three olfactometer arenas were observed simultaneously for 20 min, and mating trials were conducted in sequence immediately afterwards. Initially, there were 36 replicates tested over 5 days (data set A). One month later, 45 replicates were tested using beetles from a different culture (data set B). Experiment 2. Signal manipulation: does exposure of males to Female Factor affect mating success? The insects from the second set of 45 replicates (data set B) were immediately used in a further experiment (data set C). One male of each pair was randomly selected for exposure to Female Factor, while the other male and the female were kept separately with fresh food in a pot containing split maize grain and a small quantity of maize flour. The Female Factor treatment used an equivalent amount of maize grain from cultures 8–10 weeks old; this grain was frozen for 5 days and then allowed to equilibrate in the CTH room for 2 days. Males remained in these media treatments for 12 days. Males were tested in the same pairs as in data set B. The experiment was carried out over 3 days: – days 1 and 3 – mating trials in open arenas with females allocated at random to day and to pairs of males (15 replicates tested each day). – day 2 – pheromone bioassays in order to check the success of manipulation of pheromone signalling by exposure to Female Factor (15 replicates tested). Statistical analysis. The association of mating success with the outcome of the pheromone bioassay or the relative size of the male was tested using a chisquared test with one degree of freedom. Hypotheses about courtship behaviour in the mating trials were tested using Wilcoxon signed-ranks tests. Results Does long-range attraction predict mating success? Natural variation. Data sets A and B were used, excluding cases where there was no mating (21 cases) or the olfactometer bioassay did not reveal preference for either male (15 cases). Table 1a shows that the attractiveness of the pheromone signal did not affect the probability of being the first to mate in either data set, although there was significant heterogeneity between data sets A and B (N = 50, contingency table 2 = 3.94, P<0.05). χ(1) Manipulation of pheromone signal using Female Factor. Males exposed to fresh grain were significantly more attractive to females in olfactometer tests than males exposed to Female Factor (mean response of 3.13 compared with 0.2 visits by a female per 20 min trial: N = 30, z = 2.809, P<0.01). This result confirms that exposure to Female Factor caused males to alter their emission of aggregation pheromone in this experiment. There was, however, no significant effect of Female Factor on mating success. The male exposed to Female Factor was the first to mate in 14/26 trials and the signalling male exposed to fresh grain was the first to mate in 12/26 trials. Different females were used in mating trials B and C, yet there was strong consistency in the relative success of males in the competing pairs. Excluding the seven cases where there was no mating in either B or in C, the same male mated first in 19/23 cases and there was a change in relative success in only 4/23 cases 2 = 9.78, P<0.01). (N = 23, X(1) Does male body size predict mating success? Data sets A and B were used, excluding cases where there was no mating, and were combined because there was no heterogeneity (N = 61, contin2 = 0.19, n.s.). Table 1b shows that gency table X(1) larger males were more likely to mate first, but the effect was not statistically significant (overall N=61, 2 = 1.8, n.s.). X(1) Larger males were also visited more frequently in bioassay tests, but not significantly so (Table 1c: data sets A and B excluding cases where the olfactometer bioassay did not reveal preference for either male; 2 = 1.1, n.s.). Data sets A and overall N = 66, X(1) B could be combined because there was no hetero2 = 1.10, geneity; N = 66, contingency table X(1) n.s.). Is courtship behaviour related to mating success? Data sets A and B were used, excluding replicates where there was no mating. The data for all physical contacts between a male and a female were first separated into approaches involving the successful male (first to mate) and those involving the unsuccessful (non-mating) male, and then divided again according to whether the female approached the male or vice versa. The results for proportions of total encounters involving physical contact are plotted as separate bar charts for data sets A and B in Figure 2. 348 Table 1. The associations between males preferred by females in pheromone-signalling bioassays, their mating success in open-arena mating trials, and their relative sizes. Data sets A and B were tested separately using a chi-squared test of the hypothesis of no association. They were combined as overall figures only when a contingency-table test showed no significant heterogeneity (a) Female preference for signal and mating success Data set Preferred signaller mated first Non-preferred signaller mated first Chi-squared statistic (1 d.f.) A B 15 10 8 17 1.1 0.9 2 = 3.94, P<0.05. Test of heterogeneity: N = 50, X(1) (b) Male size and mating success Data set Heavier male mated first Lighter male mated first Chi-squared statistic (1 d.f.) A B Overall 16 22 38 11 12 23 0.5 1.5 1.8 2 = 0.19, n.s. Test of heterogeneity: N = 61, X(1) (c) Male size and female preference for signal Data set Heavier male’s signal preferred Lighter male’s signal preferred Chi-squared statistic (1 d.f.) A B Overall 21 18 39 11 16 27 1.6 0.1 1.1 2 = 1.10, n.s. Test of heterogeneity: N = 66, X(1) Figure 1. Experimental design to show which insects contributed to the three data sets, A, B and C. OB = Olfactometer bioassay, MT = Mating trial. There was no consistent or significant difference between whether the male or the female was the first to make contact; in data set A, females initiated more physical encounters while the converse was true in data set B. The three types of push in Figure 2 were compared between males that mated and males that did not mate. The only significant difference was for encounters involving no pushing, where males that mated were more likely to be involved in a physical encounter involving no pushing (A: N = 27, z = −3.39, P = 0.001; B: N = 34, z = −4.59, P<<0.001). Out of all the physical encounters in data sets A and B, 21% were between the two males. The percentage of encounters involving an obvious push by a male was an order of magnitude higher for male-male interactions (A: 30%; B: 34%) than for male-female interactions (A: 3.5%; B: 5%). Larger males made a higher mean number of pushes per trial in both sets (A: larger – 1.11, smaller – 0.39; B: larger – 1.80, 349 Figure 2. Bar charts showing the proportion of physical encounters leading to three different behaviours before mating (female pushes male, male pushes female, or no apparent pushing). Results are shown separately for the two sets of trials: (a) data set A (N = 27), (b) data set B (N = 34). smaller – 0.67 pushes per trial) but this difference was only statistically significant in data set A (A: N = 36, z = −2.20, p<0.05; B: N = 45, z = −1.59, p=0.11). The males that mated successfully made significantly more pushes than the other male in male-male interactions in set B (mated – 1.68, unmated – 0.45 pushes per trial: N = 38, z = −2.48, p=0.01) but not in set A (mated – 0.52, unmated – 0.56 pushes per trial: N = 27, z = 0.07, n.s.). Manipulating male aggregation-pheromone signalling did not result in any significant changes in courtship behaviour (comparing set B to set C) (for details see Birkinshaw, 1998). Discussion There is tremendous variation in courtship and copulation behaviour between different animal species (reviewed by Eberhard, 1996). The results described here need to be placed in the context of the normal behaviour in the plant host of LGB (Birkinshaw, 1998) and similar wood-boring beetles such as members of the Scolytidae (Kirkendall, 1983). Birkinshaw (1998) describes the reproductive behaviour of LGB both within tunnel systems and in open arenas. Outside the plant host, LGB courtship behaviour is characterised by a variable pre-copulatory period of pushing behaviour, a period of stereotyped rubbing of the female by the male using his antennae and legs, a short copulation, and little evidence of any post-copulatory interaction between male and female. In contrast, behaviour within tunnel systems is characterised by a male and a female remaining together for several days and mating repeatedly during this time. When a male has constructed a short lenth of tunnel (1–2 cm), he spends most of his time positioned at the entrance to the tunnel. Only one female cohabits with a male within his tunnel system, unlike many polygamous scolytid species (Kirkendall, 1983; Schlyter & Zhang, 1996; Schmitz, 1972). A typical sequence of behaviour resulting in mating is as follows: (1) the male approaches the female in his tunnel system and appears to entice her back into a widened region of tunnel, such as the junction of two branches or the entrance of the system, (2) the male assumes the mating position where there is enough room for copulation, (3) after mating, the female generally returns to the end of a blind-ending tunnel where she lays eggs (Li, 1988), (4) the male generally returns to the tunnel entrance. Some pushing behaviour was observed between beetles in tunnel systems and on one occasion a female was seen to expel a male by pushing. Males appear to guard the female only within a tunnel system, where the male will remain at the entrance in a manner very similar to many scolytid species; Kirkendall (1983) notes of scolytids that ‘... males that stay during gallery construction remain blocking the entrance hole, occasionally leaving their post to copulate, feed or remove accumulation of frass’. In the mating trials in open arenas described here, different females showed very similar preferences for a particular male of a pair in trials separated by 12 or more days (data sets B and C). Females were more likely to push males than vice versa. The males who were most persistent in approaching females were also most likely to secure the first mating with a female. The suggestion by Fadamiro (1995) that males are ‘the mate-finding sex’ was not convincingly supported here. In open arenas (and in tunnel systems), the movement of both males and females towards each other seemed to initiate courtship and copulation. Females appeared to exercise choice between two males by being generally reluctant to mate and thus selecting for the most persistent males. Males also pushed one another, and larger males on average made more 350 pushes per trial than smaller males (statistically significant in data set A). The males that were successful in achieving the first mating also made significantly more pushes per trial in data set B, though not in data set A. The results for males are similar to those reported by Hughes (1981) for Monochamus scutellatus (Cerambycidae), where larger males won contests more often than smaller males. Pushing behaviour is also part of male-male conflict in the Scolytidae, where it is observed on the bark surface (Kirkendall, 1983). Female choice in mating trials, however, could not be predicted either from knowledge of the relative attractiveness of pheromone signals or from relative body size. Manipulation of pheromone signalling using Female Factor confirmed that the pheromone signal did not influence female choice during close-range courtship. The data in Table 1 are from at least 50 mating trials in each case and it is therefore likely that the tests are sufficiently powerful to avoid Type II Error. All benefits of mate acquisition that result from pheromone signalling in LGB seem therefore to accrue from manipulation of dispersal and spatial distribution of females. Beetles in the field have a high resolution of perception of competing signals and can distinguish consistently between two signalling males only 0.6 m apart (Birkinshaw & Smith, 2000). Pheromone signalling could still be a major determinant of mating success when males occupy distinct tunnel systems, especially in non-grain hosts (Borgemeister et al., 1998). In maize seeds, however, the tunnel systems are neither extensive nor distinct and attraction of a female may benefit the signaller much less than in wild hosts because of the greater likelihood of interception of the female by another male. It was suggested in the Introduction that males may stop pheromone signalling when a female has been attracted as a means of reducing competition from other males. Another reason to stop signalling in the presence of a female is that it may be costly in terms of energy. Predators, however, also respond to aggregation pheromone, e.g., Teretrius nigrescens (Coleoptera: Histeridae) (Rees et al., 1990). Females and their eggs could be protected from both predators and parasitoids by males guarding the entrance of tunnel systems. There is evidence that the presence of males increases the numbers of live offspring produced through decreased predation in the Scolytidae (Kirkendall, 1983; Reid & Roitberg 1994). Protection of eggs and young larvae could be the main reason why a female stays with a single male for several days in a tunnel system while showing no such at- tachment in an open arena. Increased risk of offspring predation from males that continued to signal may, therefore, be one reason why females use cues other than pheromone signalling to choose between mates once a male/males have been located. Finally, we consider whether there are any consequences for pest management. The implications of knowledge of mating systems and sexual selection for pest management have been reviewed by Boake et al. (1996). In LGB, dispersing beetles are sensitive to natural variation in aggregation-pheromone signalling. Manipulation of host selection using aggregation pheromone is therefore possible (Birkinshaw & Smith, 2000). The results reported here, however, show that short-range courtship behaviour is insensitive to pheromone signal. Aggregation pheromone cannot therefore be used as a courtship disruptant in this species. Acknowledgements This publication is an output from a research project funded by the United Kingdom Department for International Development (DFID) for the benefit of developing countries. The views expressed are not necessarily those of DFID. R6201 ‘Crop Post Harvest Research Programme’. References Andersson, M., 1994. Sexual Selection. Monographs in Behaviour and Ecology. Princeton University Press, New Jersey. 559 pp. Birkinshaw, L. A., 1998. Mate choice in Prostephanus truncatus (Horn) (Coleoptera: Bostrichidae): The role of male-produced aggregation pheromone. Ph.D. thesis, University of Leicester. Birkinshaw, L. A. & R. H. Smith, 2000. Function of aggregation pheromone in the larger grain borer Prostephanus truncatus: Variation in response to individuals as evidence for a role in sexual selection. Journal of Chemical Ecology 26: 1325–1339. Boake, C. R. B., T. E. Shelly & K. Y. Kaneshiro, 1996. Sexual selection in relation to pest-management strategies. Annual Review of Entomology 41: 211–229. Borgemeister, C., F. Djossou, C. Adda, H. Schneider, B. Djomamou, P. Degbey, B. Azoma & R. H. Markham, 1997a. Establishment, spread, and impact of Teretriosoma nigrescens (Coleoptera: Histeridae), an exotic predator of the larger grain borer (Coleoptera: Bostrichidae) in southwestern Benin. Environmental Entomology 26: 1405–1415. Borgemeister, C., W. G. Meikle, D. Scholz, C. Adda, P. Degbey & R. H. Markham, 1997b. Seasonal and weather factors influencing the annual flight cycle of Prostephanus truncatus (Coleoptera:Bostrichidae) and its predator Teretriosoma nigrescens (Coleoptera:Histeridae) in Benin. Bulletin of Entomological Research 87: 239–246. 351 Borgemeister, C., A. Tchabi & D. Scholz, 1998. Trees or stores? The origin of migrating Prostephanus truncatus collected in different ecological habitats in southern Benin. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 87: 285–294. Cork, A., D. R. Hall, R. J. Hodges & J. A. Pickett, 1991. Identification of major component of male-produced aggregation pheromone of larger grain borer, Prostephanus truncatus (Horn) (Coleoptera: Bostrichdiae). Journal of Chemical Ecology 17: 789–803. Crespi, B. J., 1989. Causes of assortative mating in arthropods. Animal Behaviour 38: 980–1000. Eberhard, W. G., 1996. Female Control: Sexual Selection by Cryptic Female Choice. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey. Fadamiro, H. Y., 1995. Flight behaviour and pheromone communication of the Larger Grain Borer, Prostephanus truncatus (Horn) (Coleoptera: Bostrichidae). Ph.D. thesis, University of Oxford. Hodges, R. J., 1994. Recent advances in the biology and control of Prostephanus truncatus (Coleoptera: Bostrichidae). Proceedings of the 6th International Working Conference on Stored-Product Protection 2: 929–934. Hodges, R. J., 1996. The biology and control of Prostephanus truncatus (Horn) (Coleoptera: Bostrichidae) – a destructive storage pest with an increasing range. Journal of Stored Products Research 22: 1–14. Holloway, G. J. & R. H. Smith, 1987. Sexual selection of body weight in Sitophilus oryzae (L.) (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Journal of Stored Products Research 23: 197–202. Hughes, A. L., 1981. Differential male mating success in the white spotted sawyer Monochamus scutellatus (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae). Annals of the Entomological Society of America. Kirkendall, L. R., 1983. The evolution of mating systems in bark and ambrosia beetles (Coleoptera: Scolytidae and Platypodidae). Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society 77: 293–352. Li, L., 1988. Behavioural ecology and life history evolution in the larger grain borer, Prostephanus truncatus (Horn). Ph.D. thesis, University of Reading. Moore, A. J., 1998. Female preferences, male social status, and sexual selection in Nauphoeta cinerea. Animal Behaviour 36: 303–305. Moore, A. J., N. L. Reagan & K. F. Haynes, 1995. Conditional signalling strategies: effects on ontogeny, social experience and social status on the pheromonal signal of male cockroaches. Animal Behaviour 50: 191–202. Moore, P. J., N. L. Reagan-Wallin, K. F. Haynes & A. J. Moore, 1997. Odour conveys status on cockroaches. Nature 389: 25–25. Rees, D. P., R. Rodriguez Rivera & F. J., Herrera Rodriguez, 1990. Observations on the ecology of Teretriosoma nigrescens Lewis (Col.: Histeridae) and its prey Prostephanus truncatus (Horn) (Col.: Bostrichidae) in the Yucatan peninsula, Mexico. Tropical Science 30: 153–165. Reid, M. L. & B. D. Roitberg, 1994. Benefits of prolonged male residence with mates and brood in pine engravers (Coleoptera: Scolytidae). Oikos 70: 140–148. Schlyter, F. & Q.-H. Zhang, 1996. Testing avian polygyny hypotheses in insects: harem size distribution and female egg gallery spacing in three Ips bark beetles. Oikos 76: 57–69. Schmitz, R. F., 1972. Behaviour of Ips pini during mating, oviposition, and larval development (Coleoptera: Scolytidae). The Canadian Entomologist 104: 1723–1728. Scholz, D., C. Borgemeister, W. G. Meikle, R. H. Markham & H. M. Poehling, 1997. Infestation of maize by Prostephanus truncatus initiated by male-produced pheromone. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 83: 53–61. Smith, J. L., A. Cork, D. R. Hall & R. J. Hodges, 1996. Investigation of the effect of female larger grain borer, Prostephanus truncatus (Horn) (Coleoptera: Bostrichidae), and their residues on the production of aggregation pheromone by males. Journal of Stored Products Research 32: 171–181.