Innovations in Education and Teaching International ISSN: 1470-3297 (Print) 1470-3300 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/riie20 Debate: A new approach for improving the dialectical thinking of university students Xiaoshan Li, Zaichao Han, Jianping Fu, Yun Mei & Jianping Liu To cite this article: Xiaoshan Li, Zaichao Han, Jianping Fu, Yun Mei & Jianping Liu (2019): Debate: A new approach for improving the dialectical thinking of university students, Innovations in Education and Teaching International, DOI: 10.1080/14703297.2019.1640123 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/14703297.2019.1640123 Published online: 16 Jul 2019. Submit your article to this journal View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=riie20 INNOVATIONS IN EDUCATION AND TEACHING INTERNATIONAL https://doi.org/10.1080/14703297.2019.1640123 Debate: A new approach for improving the dialectical thinking of university students Xiaoshan Lia,b, Zaichao Hanc, Jianping Fuc, Yun Meid and Jianping Liua,b a School of Psychology, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China; bCenter of mental health education and research, School of Psychology, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China; cCollege of Life Science, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China; dSchool of Geography and Environment, Jiangxi Normal University, Nanchang, China ABSTRACT KEYWORDS Although dialectical thinking plays an important role in daily life, this ability among adults, including university students, is not always well developed. The present study used three experimental designs to explore efficient ways to promote university students’ dialectical thinking ability. The results showed that university students’ dialectical thinking ability can be improved after receiving instruction according to specific teaching practices. The debate approach was a more interesting and efficient teaching method for cultivating dialectical thinking ability than the lecture approach. These findings broaden and deepen our understanding about the development of dialectical thinking ability and suggests that debate can be used as an alternative way, in many university courses or training projects, to improve students’ dialectical thinking ability. Debate teaching approach; dialectical thinking; lecture teaching approach Introduction Dialectical thinking is a cognitive process and epistemological method for grappling with contradictions (Peng & Nisbett, 1999). It holds that everything is changeable and interrelated and tolerates contradiction (e.g. ‘good’ and ‘bad’ can exist in the same object or event) (Bai, 2015). Dialectical thinking ability is one of many thinking faculties and plays an important role in daily life. For example, it has been found to be beneficial for interpreting contradictions around the world (Lomas & Ivtzan, 2015) and promoting individual social adaptation (e.g. Wu & Lin, 2005). Bai (2015) also found that leaders with high dialectical thinking scores promote employee creativity and in-role performance by encouraging team members to produce a wider set of diverse ideas and more thoughtful solutions to solve their problems. However, research has shown that the dialectical thinking ability of adults, including university students, is not well-developed (e.g. Basseches, 2005; Cheng, 2009; Zhang, Gao, & Hua, 2011) and less attention is paid to how to cultivate this ability. In response to this issue, two questions should be answered. The first is whether dialectical thinking can be improved by pedagogical practices at the university course level. Secondly, if dialectical thinking can be improved by CONTACT Jianping Fu [email protected] Number 99, ZiYang Road, Nanchang 330022, China © 2019 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group College of Life Science, Jiangxi Normal University, 2 X. LI ET AL. the implementation of certain pedagogical practices, which teaching approach most efficiently improves individual thinking ability? As ‘thinking ability’ denotes a cognitive faculty, it can be improved through daily learning activities, and there is substantial evidence showing that students can become better thinkers by appropriate instruction (e.g. Arrue, Unanue, & Merida, 2017; Danielson, Preast, Bender, & Hassall, 2014; Halpern, 1998). Therefore, there is a reason to believe that the dialectical thinking ability of university students can be improved through the implementation of appropriate teaching practices in university courses. Lecture and debate are two main teaching approaches for cultivating individual thinking ability (Arrue et al., 2017). The ‘lecture approach’ refers to a teaching-learning strategy in which a teacher demonstrates the knowledge or skills that are to be learned through formal in-class presentations or occasional interaction (e.g. Students were asked to answer the clicker questions in a predominantly lecture-based context) (Danielson et al., 2014). In a lecture class, the key element is the teacher. The ‘debate approach’ refers to a teaching-learning strategy that presupposes an established position (either pro or con) on an issue, assertion, proposition, or solution to a problem (Arrue et al., 2017). As a key method by which students learn in a debate-style class, dialogue is proved to be beneficial to promote students’ thinking ability (Kuhn & Crowell, 2011; Walton, 1989). Therefore, we tend to believe that debate is a more efficient teaching approach for cultivating university students’ dialectical thinking ability than lecture for the following reasons. First, unlike most lecture classes, which focus on content knowledge (Halpern, 1998), debate classes tend to focus on transferability of thinking skills (e.g. to ‘win’ the debate using logical expression), which can contribute to the development of an individual’s cognitive faculties, including dialectical thinking. Second, teachers in lecture-style classes engage students through formal in-class presentations or occasional interaction. However, the teachers, as well as most adults, are biased in their interpretations of the objective world around them based on prior beliefs (Hilbert, 2012). Such bias might serve as a barrier to students’ dialectical thinking ability development in a lecture class setting. However, this issue can be solved in a debate class due to the participants’ complementary knowledge regarding complex social issues (Li, Zhou, Zhao, Zhang, & Zhang, 2015). Third, students in a debate class have more opportunities than those in a lecture class to advocate their opinions while simultaneously acknowledging the opposition’s arguments (Arrue et al., 2017); both of these skills are beneficial to the development of dialectical thinking ability. Accordingly, in this study, the following hypotheses were proposed: Hypothesis 1: Debate can improve students’ dialectical thinking ability. Hypothesis 2: Debate-based class is more effective than lecture-based class in cultivating university students’ dialectical thinking ability. Three studies were used to test the above hypotheses. Study 1 aimed to determine whether students tended to think more ‘dialectically’ after receiving instruction according to a specific teaching practice (e.g. debate). Studies 2 and 3 aimed to test the efficiency of different teaching approaches for cultivating individual dialectical thinking ability using two different measurement indices of dialectical thinking. With the approval of the Ethical Review Board of the Jiangxi Normal University, all three studies were undertaken after participants had given formal consent. INNOVATIONS IN EDUCATION AND TEACHING INTERNATIONAL 3 Study 1 Participants and procedure Participants included 48 undergraduates (40 women and 8 men; age: M = 19.2, SD = .36) who were taking part in the ‘positive psychology’ curriculum at Jiangxi Normal University. Most were first- or second-year students. At the beginning of the semester, students were evenly divided into 12 groups with designated debate topics (e.g. the ‘against’ or ‘for’ position for six debate topics) and engaged in a debate during the last few weeks of the semester. The six debate topics were as follows: ‘Be tactful in life’, ‘Forgiveness is a panacea for a healthy society’, ‘If we love her/him, we ought to do favours for her/him unconditionally’, ‘Life needs to be well planned’, ‘We need charity’, and ‘Cooperation is more conductive to our learning than competition’. Participants were asked to offer their views on the topics before and after the debates. All items in the present study were scored on a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from ‘Strongly Disagree’ (1) to ‘Strongly Agree’ (7). All participants completed their participation in the study and none were excluded. The analytic method One important characteristic of dialectical thinking is the so-called ‘doctrine of the mean’, in which people tend to offer his/her view to the issue moderately rather than in either extreme (e.g. strongly agree or disagree) (Peng & Nisbett, 1999). In present study, point 4 in 7-point Likert scale means a moderate response to an issue. Therefore, students’ dialectical thinking ability was measured according to the pre/post-mid distance, which was computed using the following formula: pre/post-mid distance = |pretest/post-test scores – 4|. The paired-sample t-test was used to examine the difference between pre- and post-mid distances. Results Table 1 shows that students had lower post-mid distance scores than the pre-mid distance scores for all debate topics, except T1 and T2 (ps. < .05), indicating that students tended to think dialectically after receiving instruction according to a specific pedagogical approach (e.g. debate), which supported Hypothesis 1. Table 1. The changes of students’ view on debate issues before and after debate. The students’ view on debate issues (M ± SD) T1 T2 T3 T4 T5 T6 Pre-mid distance 1.44 ± 1.07 1.71 ± .99 1.69 ± .99 1.56 ± .94 1.65 ± .86 1.63 ± .84 Post-mid distance 1.17 ± .81 1.38 ± 1.00 1.25 ± 1.0 1.21 ± .99 1.25 ± .81 1.27 ± .87 t 1.83 1.70 2.60** 2.15* 2.67** 2.23* Cohen’d .26 .25 .38 .31 .38 .32 Note: *p < .05, **p < .01. T1 = ‘Be tactful in life’, T2 = ‘forgiveness is a panacea for health society’, T3 = ‘if we love her/him, we ought to do favor for her/him unconditionally’, T4 = ‘life need well plan’, T5 = ‘we need charity’, T6 = ‘cooperation is more conductive to our learning than competition’. 4 X. LI ET AL. The results of Study 1 showed that university students tended to think dialectically after receiving instruction according to specific teaching practices, but the efficacy of the different teaching approaches for cultivating this thinking ability was still unknown. Therefore, we designed two additional studies (Study 2 and Study 3) to investigate the effects of the different teaching approaches (debate vs. lecture) on an individual’s dialectical thinking ability. Study 2 Participants Against the backdrop of university curriculum reform in China, implementation of new teaching approaches was encouraged. Students enrolled in two ‘happiness psychology’ classes at Jiangxi Normal University were chosen as the sample for this study. The first class consisted of 62 first-year students (28 females) majoring in Mathematics and Information Science; they were designated to the ‘lecture class’ group of the study. The second class consisted of 65 first-year students (27 females) majoring in Electronic Information Science; they were designated to the ‘debate class’ group of the study. No significant difference according to gender was found for the two classes (χ2 = .24, p > .05). Two students in the lecture class and one in the debate class were excluded from the study because of their prior debate experience. In total, the data from 60 students in the lecture class and 64 students in the debate class were used in the following analysis. Research materials Dialectical thinking in this study was measured by the Zhong-Yong Thinking Style Scale, which was compiled by Chiu (2000) based on the characteristics of dialectical thinking (e.g. integration, mean tendency) and adapted by Wu and Lin (2005). It consists of three sub-scales: diversification (i.e. considering things carefully from different aspects), integrity (i.e. integrating one’s perspectives with others’), and harmony (i.e. acting in a manner that maintains interpersonal harmony). The scale has been proved to have good reliability and validity in predicting individual dialectical thinking ability in a Chinese sample (Zhang et al., 2011). Each item is scored on a 7-point Likerttype scale ranging from ‘Strongly Disagree’ (1) to ‘Strongly Agree’ (7). In the current study, the Cronbach’s α of the three subscales ranged from .69 to .73, and the Cronbach’s α of the entire scale was .84. Interestingness/usefulness of curriculum was measured by the item ‘I find this curriculum very interesting/useful’, and scored using a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from ‘Strongly Disagree’ (1) to ‘Strongly Agree’ (5). Research procedure At the beginning and end of the semester, students from both the debate and lecture classes were asked to complete the dialectical thinking questionnaires. Students from both classes were also asked to report their views on the usefulness INNOVATIONS IN EDUCATION AND TEACHING INTERNATIONAL 5 and interestingness of this curriculum and their experiences with debate outside the class at the end of the semester. To gain a better understanding of the impact of the two teaching approaches on individual dialectical thinking ability, teachers’ characteristics and course contents were considered in the experimental design. Two male psychology teachers, whom a teaching evaluation team at the beginning of the semester had rated as average for ‘personal charm’ and ‘teaching ability’, were selected and, respectively, assigned to lead the lecture and debate classes. Regarding course contents, the ‘happy psychology’ teaching team designed a ‘tight’ curriculum to ensure that students in both classes would learn the same contents at the same teaching pace. That is, teachers in both classes instructed the students on the theory and practice of happiness during the first 10 weeks of the semester and the ‘six humanistic topics’ (i.e. love, plan, cooperation, forgiveness, charity, and interpersonal relations) in the last six weeks. The only difference was that the debate teaching approach was used in the debate class to learn the six humanistic topics through the six debate topics listed in Study 1, while the lecture teaching approach was used in the lecture class for the last six weeks. In the lecture class, students learned relative knowledge about the six humanistic topics (i.e. concepts or related theories) through the teacher’s formal in-class presentations or occasional interactions. The analytic method We hypothesised that first-year students in both classes would show no significant difference in dialectical thinking ability due to their similar learning experience before entering university. However, we also hypothesised that, after practising these skills, the students in the debate class would be more likely to think dialectically at the end of the semester. Thus, we expected there to be an interaction between teaching approaches and measuring times. To test this, multivariate repeated measures (ANOVA method) were used to examine the effect of measuring times (pre-test vs. post-test) and teaching approaches (debate vs. lecture) on students’ dialectical thinking ability. Results The means and standard deviations calculated for these analyses are presented in Table 2. The ANOVA results showed that students in the debate class scored high on the subscales and that their overall score signalling dialectical thinking ability was higher than that of their peers in the lecture class. Diversification analysis The results of the diversification analysis showed that both the main effect of the teaching approaches (F[1, 122] = 3.17, p = .08, partial eta2 = .03) and the interaction ‘teaching approaches × measuring times’ (F[1, 122] = 2.76, p = .09, partial eta2 = .02) were marginally significant. The independent-samples t-test showed that, at the beginning of the semester (t[122] = 1.26, p > .1), there was no difference in the means of the diversification scores between the debate class (M = 5.03) and lecture class (M = 4.87). 6 X. LI ET AL. Table 2. Means and standard deviations of dependent measures in lecture and debate class condition at the beginning and end of the semester. At the beginning of the semester M(SD) Measure Diversification Integrity Harmony Total score Lecture 4.87(.72) 5.23(.66) 5.01(.66) 5.05(.52) Debate 5.03(.69) 5.18(.53) 5.17(.63) 5.13(.45) At the end of the semester M(SD) Lecture 5.09(.91) 5.28(.88) 5.1(.87) 5.17(.72) Debate 5.4(.78) 5.48(.63) 5.53(.69) 5.47(.55) However, at the end of the semester (t[122] = 2.05, p = .04), students in the debate class exhibited a higher mean score in diversification (M = 5.4) than their peers in the lecture class (M = 5.09). Integrity analysis The results of the integrity analysis showed no main effect in terms of teaching approach (F[1, 122] = .40, p > .1). However, the predicted ‘teaching approaches × measuring times’ interaction was significant (F[1, 122] = 9.44, p = .003, partial eta2 = .07). The paired-samples t-test showed that students in the lecture class exhibited no difference in integrity scores at the beginning (M = 5.23) or the end of the semester (M = 5.28), t(59) = .75, p > .1. However, students in the debate class exhibited higher integrity scores at the end of the semester (M = 5.48) than they did at the beginning ([M = 5.18], t[63] = 5.27, p < .001). Harmony analysis The results of the harmony analysis showed that the main effect of the teaching method (F[1, 122] = 6.01, p = .01, partial eta2 = .05) and the predicted ‘teaching approaches × measuring times’ interaction was found to be significant, (F[1, 122] = 8.63, p = .004, partial eta2 = .07). The independent-samples t-test showed that there was no difference in the harmony score between the debate class (M = 5.17) and the lecture class (M = 5.01) at the beginning of the semester (t[122] = 1.41, p > .1). However, students in the debate class showed higher scores for harmony (M = 5.53) than those in the lecture class (M = 5.1) at the end of the semester (t[122] = 3.05, p < .01). Analysis of the overall score for dialectical thinking The results for the overall score signalling dialectical thinking showed that the main effect of the teaching approaches was marginally significant (F[1, 122] = 3.85, p = .052, partial eta2 = .04); however, the predicted ‘teaching approaches × measuring times interaction’ was found to be significant (F[1, 122] = 15.08, p = .000, partial eta2 = .11). The independent-samples t-test showed that there was no difference in the overall scores signifying dialectical thinking ability between the debate class (M = 5.13) and the lecture class (M = 5.05) at the beginning of the semester (t[122] = .89, p > .1); however, students in the debate class demonstrated a higher overall score for dialectical thinking ability (M = 5.47) than those in the lecture class (M = 5.17) at the end of the semester (t[122] = 2.64, p < .01). INNOVATIONS IN EDUCATION AND TEACHING INTERNATIONAL 7 Study 3 Participants and research materials Many proverbs, particularly those regarding the particular condition of human wisdom, are full of contradiction; therefore, they are often used as experimental material when assessing an individual’s dialectical thinking ability (Peng & Nisbett, 1999). Thus, the edited proverb, ‘Love me, love all of mine unconditionally’ was used as experimental material to measure dialectical thinking in the present study. This proverb was modified from two proverbs, ‘love me, love my dog’ and ‘demonstrate love and care unconditionally’ (Holloman & Yates, 2013). Five psychology graduate students were invited to write down their feeling about this edited proverb, and they all agreed that the proverb inherently contained a contradiction. The same sample that took part in Study 2 participated in Study 3. Three students were excluded on account of their prior experience with debate, and another student was excluded because he did not fully complete the questionnaire. Data from a total of 60 students from the lecture class and 63 from the debate class were utilised in the following analysis. Instruction in the lecture and debate classes followed the same syllabus and procedure as Study 2, except that all participants were asked to write down their views on a particular issue rather than respond to the dialectical thinking scale. At the end of the semester, all participants were asked to write down their views of the edited proverb. Students’ responses were then coded as either ‘dialectical’ or ‘non-dialectical’. The analytic method Careful consideration of a matter according to its different aspects and tolerance of contradiction are the most important characteristics of dialectical thinking (Wu & Lin, 2005). To analyse participants’ responses, a coding method (Peng & Nisbett, 1999) was used to distinguish ‘dialectical’ from ‘non-dialectical’ responses. A ‘dialectical response’ was defined as one that (a) addressed the issue from both contradicting sides and (b) attempted to reconcile the conflict by compromising. In the present study, one dialectical response written by a student was as follows: ‘the belief that “love all of mine unconditionally” is a two-edged sword. On the one hand, it has a positive relation with the person’s mental health through acceptance of their imperfections; on the other hand, people who are too insistent on accepting their imperfection (e.g. bad habits or dangerous behaviours) will hurt themselves or people around them and are easily regarded as arrogant and selfish men/women. So, it is worth it to have this belief only if it doesn’t have a conflict with a social form’. Non-dialectical responses generally argued exclusively for one side or the other. Examples of non-dialectical responses written in this study are as follows: ‘I agree with this belief due to its positive relation to high life satisfaction’, or ‘I disagree with the belief that people who are too insistent on accepting imperfection (e.g. bad habits or dangerous behaviours) will hurt themselves or people around them and are easily regarded as arrogant and selfish men/women’. Students’ responses were coded as ‘dialectical’ or ‘non-dialectical’ by two coders with an inter-coder reliability of .94 as indicated by the Pearson correlation. Regarding responses 8 X. LI ET AL. where the two coders diverged in their opinion about the student’s degree of dialectical thinking, the two coders discussed the student’s view until a consensus was reached. To reduce the effect of rater bias, a double-blind experimental design was used. Results Figure 1 shows that students in both the lecture and debate classes preferred dialectical thinking in response to the proverb. In the debate class, more students’ responses were coded as ‘dialectical’ (76%) than ‘non-dialectical’ (24%) (z [n = 63] = 8.32, p < .001). A similar result was found in the lecture class: more responses were coded as ‘dialectical’ (59%) than ‘non-dialectical’ (41%) (z [n = 60] = 2.58, p < .01). Finally, more students in the debate class (76%) were coded as ‘dialectical thinkers’ than those in the lecture class (59%) (z [n = 123] = 2.11, p < .05). Discussion Increasing empirical studies offer support for the important role of dialectical thinking ability in this era of explosive information. However, it has been reported (e.g. Bai, 2015; Zhang et al., 2011) that most adults, including university students, have not fully developed their dialectical thinking ability. As cultivation of this ability may be important for human development (Basseches, 2005), the present study aimed to find an efficient teaching approach for cultivating the dialectical thinking ability of university students. Figure 1. The effect of different teaching approaches on students’ dialectical thinking. Note: **p < .01. The Vertical Bar represents the percentage of participants in debate or lecture class were coded as ‘dialectical’ or ‘non-dialectical’ thinkers. INNOVATIONS IN EDUCATION AND TEACHING INTERNATIONAL 9 University teaching practices can improve students’ dialectical thinking ability Students in Study 1 showed less post-mid distance than pre-mid distance of their views on four out of six debate issues, indicating that they were more likely to think dialectically after receiving instruction according to a specific teaching practice, namely, debate (which supports Hypothesis 1). This implies that dialectical thinking ability is one of many important cognitive faculties which can be improved through proper instruction and practice, a finding consistent with Halpern’s view (Halpern, 1998). One possible explanation for this might be that a participant’s complementary knowledge regarding complex social issues increases their understanding about a debate topic, thus reducing ‘black and white’ thinking about a topic as well as personal bias. Debate was a more effective teaching approach for cultivating university students’ dialectical thinking ability than lecture Studies 2 and 3 used quasi-experimental designs to test the effects of different teaching strategies on students’ dialectical thinking ability, taking into consideration teachers’ characteristics and course contents. The findings showed that the debate-style class was more effective for cultivating university students’ dialectical thinking ability than lectures (Supporting Hypothesis 2). This could be due to the different focal points of the two teaching approaches: Lecture classes focus on understanding knowledge content, while debate classes focus on how to win a debate about a particular issue using logical thinking. Compared with students in a lecture class, in a debate class, students must consider things carefully from different aspects if they are to win the debate (Wu & Lin, 2005). In addition, non-debaters or ‘watchers’ in a debate class have opportunities to further develop their dialectical thinking ability by observing how their peers and teachers think about and debate social issues (Arrue et al., 2017). In fact, a teacher’s summary of debate issues often includes dialectical thinking. For example, one of teachers in present study offered his summary as “Each issue, especially for complex social issues, has its strength and weakness.’ Third, participants’ complementary knowledge about various issues could reduce their bias or ignorance when it comes to interpreting the objective world based on their prior experience. Just as one student in the debate class said: ‘This debate overturns my view on forgiveness. I have heard many voices from our teachers, parents and friends speak about the benefits of forgiveness, which is of benefit to you, to me, to our society. Here, I hear some different voices from some students. That is, forgiveness is not a panacea; it also has a dark side, which likes the devil sometimes’. In addition, students in the debate class showed more interest than those in the lecture class in Study 2. One possible explanation for this is that debate is an efficient way to promote individual critical thinking and oral communication abilities (Arrue et al., 2017), which in turn increase students’ active involvement in class and interest in learning. We also found that students in both the lecture and debate classes in Study 3 tended to think dialectically in response to the proverb, indicating that Chinese participants preferred to think about an issue from different sides and opted for compromising solutions to conflicts, a finding consistent with Peng and Nisbett (1999). 10 X. LI ET AL. Implications As an important cognitive ability, dialectical thinking plays an important role in university students’ daily lives. However, this ability has not been well developed, and how to cultivate it remains an important issue for university education. In response to this issue, we undertook this study using three different experimental designs. The findings showed that university students’ dialectical thinking ability could be improved through appropriate teaching practices, that is, debate may be a more efficient teaching approach than lecture for cultivating this ability. These findings can help broaden understanding about the development of individual dialectical thinking ability, especially regarding the impact of university-level teaching practices and approaches to dialectical thinking ability. Also, these findings have important practical implications for educationalists and policymakers. Namely, that debate can be used as an alternative method in many courses or training projects to improve university students’ dialectical thinking ability. Limitations The present study had some limitations. First, participants in Study 1 did not tend to think dialectically after debating two of the six topics, and some participants did not change their view on the topics even after the debate. This implies that the effectiveness of the debate teaching approach may be limited by the debate’s context and personal characteristics (e.g. motivation). Therefore, more research should be undertaken to investigate the possible moderating factors between debate teaching approaches and students’ dialectical thinking ability. Second, we found that, in practice, debate as a teaching approach has some shortcomings. For example, students often experience a high level of anxiety when facing an opponent in debate class. Furthermore, some students are only concerned about the topic in which they are taking part, to the detriment of the debate’s effectiveness (Temple, 2010). Thus, future studies should focus on exploring an efficient way to reduce these negative effects. In addition, the positive effect of the debate teaching approach for developing students’ dialectical thinking ability was only examined in terms of the discipline of psychology. More evidence should, therefore, be examined within the frameworks of other disciplines, such as education and sociology. Conclusion During a time when discourse about university education is paying attention to innovative teaching approaches that foster students’ thinking ability and active learning processes, the debate approach has received serious consideration. Our findings showed that using debate as a teaching approach permitted students to become actively involved in learning course content while promoting dialectical thinking ability. This investigation broadened and deepened our understanding about how university students’ dialectical thinking ability develops, and shed light on how to better cultivate this ability through the university curriculum, especially in terms of explaining complex social issues to students using debate. INNOVATIONS IN EDUCATION AND TEACHING INTERNATIONAL 11 Disclosure statement No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. Funding This work was supported by the social science foundation of Jiangxi province (Grant number 16BJ21); Teaching Reform Program of Jiangxi province (Grant number JXJG-18-2-31); the University humanities program of Jiangxi province (Grant number XL17205); Teaching Reform Program of Jiangxi Normal University (Grant number JXSDJG1701); the educational science foundation of Jiangxi Province (Grant number 16YB033). Notes on contributors Xiaoshan Li is a lecturer at school of psychology, Jiangxi Normal University, China. His research focuses on the issues such as China’s traditional culture, occupational health, Educational Technology, and E-Learning. Zaichao Han is an instructor at the College of Life Science, Jiangxi Normal University, China. His research focuses on college students’ mental health and career planning. Jianping Fu is a lecturer at the College of Life Science, Jiangxi Normal University, China. Her research interests are Educational Technology, Collaborative Learning, and Computer Supported Collaborative Learning. Yun Mei is an assistant professor at School of Geography and Environment, Jiangxi Normal University, China. His research focuses on college students’ mental health and career planning. Jianping Liu is a professor at school of psychology, Jiangxi Normal University, China. His research focuses on improving our understanding of the teaching and learning of science. References Arrue, M., Unanue, S., & Merida, D. (2017). Guided university debate: Effect of a new teaching-learning strategy for undergraduate nursing students. Nurse Education Today, 59, 26–32. Bai, Y. (2015). Good and bad simultaneously?: Leaders using dialectical thinking foster positive conflict and employee performance. International Journal of Conflict Management, 26, 245–267. Basseches, M. (2005). The development of dialectical thinking as an approach to integration. Integral Review, 1, 47–63. Cheng, C. (2009). Dialectical thinking and coping flexibility: A multimethod approach. Journal of Personality, 77, 471–494. Chiu, C. Y. (2000). Assessment of Zhong-Yong (dialectical) thinking. Preliminary findings from a cross-regional study. Hong Kong Journal of Social Sciences, 18, 33−55. [赵志裕. (2000). 中庸思维 的测量: 一项跨地区研究的初步结果. 香港社会科学学报, 18, 33–55.]. Danielson, J., Preast, V., Bender, H., & Hassall, L. (2014). Is the effectiveness of lecture capture related to teaching approach or content type? Computers & Education, 72, 121–131. Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains. Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 53, 449–455. Hilbert, M. (2012). Toward a synthesis of cognitive biases: How noisy information processing can bias human decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 211–237. Holloman, H., & Yates, P. H. (2013). Cloudy with a chance of sarcasm or sunny with high expectations: Using best practice language to strengthen positive behavior intervention and support efforts. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 15, 124–127. 12 X. LI ET AL. Kuhn, D., & Crowell, A. (2011). Dialogic argumentation as a vehicle for developing young adolescents’ thinking. Psychological Science, 22, 545–552. Li, X., Zhou, M., Zhao, N., Zhang, S., & Zhang, J. (2015). Collective-efficacy as a mediator of the relationship of leaders’ personality traits and team performance: A cross-level analysis. International Journal of Psychology, 50, 223–231. Lomas, T., & Ivtzan, I. (2015). Second wave positive psychology: Exploring the positive–Negative dialectics of wellbeing. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 1–16. Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–754. Temple, M. A. (2010). Using debate to develop health literacy. Journal of School Health, 67, 116–117. Walton, D. N. (1989). Dialogue theory for critical thinking. Argumentation, 3, 169–184. Wu, C. H., & Lin, Y. C. (2005). Development of a zhong-yong thinking style scale. Indigenous Psychology Research in Chinese Society, 24, 247–300. Zhang, X. Y., Gao, D. G., & Hua, F. U. (2011). Dialectical thinking reduces aggressive tendencies. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 43, 42–51.